Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T14:27:11.072Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotional contagion in children with autism spectrum disorder varies with stimulus familiarity and task instructions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 March 2019

Molly S. Helt*
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, Trinity College, Hartford, CT, USA
Deborah A. Fein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA
Jacob E. Vargas
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Trinity College, Hartford, CT, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Molly S. Helt, Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, Trinity College, 300 Summit St., Hartford, CT01606; E-mail: molly.helt@trincoll.edu.

Abstract

Although deficits in cognitive empathy are well established in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), the literature on emotional empathy, or emotional contagion, in individuals with ASD is sparse and contradictory. The authors tested susceptibility to contagious yawning and laughter in children with ASD (n = 60) and typically developing (TD) children (n = 60), ages 5–17 years, under various conditions, to elucidate factors that may affect emotional contagion in these populations. Although TD children showed equal amounts of emotional contagion across conditions, children with ASD were highly influenced by the familiarity of the target stimulus, as well as task instructions that encourage eye gaze to target. More specifically, children with ASD exhibited less contagious yawning and laughter than their TD peers except when their attention was explicitly directed to the eyes or (and even more so) when their parents served as the stimulus targets. The authors explore the implications of these findings for theories about the mechanisms underlying empathic deficits in ASD as well as the clinical implications of having parents involved in treatment.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasson, P., & Dimberg, U. (2008). Emotional empathy and facial feedback. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 215224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnott, S. R., Singhal, A., & Goodale, M. A. (2009). An investigation of auditory contagious yawning. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 335342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avenanti, A., Paluello, I. M., Bufalari, I., & Aglioti, S. M. (2006). Stimulus-driven modulation of motor-evoked potentials during observation of others’ pain. Neuroimage, 32, 316324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bacon, A. L., Fein, D., Morris, R., & Waterhouse, L. (1998). The responses of autistic children to the distress of others. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 129142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A fifteen year review. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 320). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beall, P. M., Moody, E. J., McIntosh, D. N., Hepburn, S. L., & Reed, C. L. (2008). Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 206223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64, 228237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 135145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundv, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 249261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences USA, 100, 54975502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C., Hofstetter, C., & Vuilleumier, P. (2011). Felt and seen pain evoke the same local patterns of cortical activity in insular and cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1799618006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H.H., … Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. (1996). Empathy: A social psychological approach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Ring, R. H. (2012). Social attention: A possible early indicator of efficacy in autism clinical trials. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 4, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. (2004). The functional architecture of human empath. Behavioral Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Gelder, B., & Hadjikhani, N. (2006). Non-conscious recognition of emotional body language. Neuroreport, 17, 583586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deputte, B. L. (1994). Ethological study of yawning in primates: Quantitative analysis and study of causation in 2 species of Old-World monkeys (Cercopithecus albigena and Macaca fascicularis). Ethology, 98, 221245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome: Using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 464473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estow, S., Jamieson, J. P., & Yates, J. (2007). Self-monitoring and mimicry of positive and negative social behaviors. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 425433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fein, D. A., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Braverman, M., & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Toward a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 2, 198212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology Letters, 16, 18241829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giganti, F., & Esposito Ziello, M. (2009). Contagious and spontaneous yawning in autistic and typically developing children. Current Psychology Letters, 25, 112.Google Scholar
Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 693705. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gu, X., Eilam-Stock, T., Zhou, T., Anagnostou, E., Kolevzon, A., Soorya, L., … Fan, J. (2015). Autonomic and brain responses associated with empathy deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 36, 33233338. doi:10.1002/hbm.22840CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Manoach, D. S., Naik, P., Snyder, J., Dominick, K., … de Gelder, B. (2009). Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 7078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Steele, S., … Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. NeuroImage, 22, 11411150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadjikhani, N., Zürcher, N. R., Rogier, O., Hippolyte, L., Lemonnier, E., Ruest, T., … Prkachin, K. M. (2014). Emotional contagion for pain is intact in autism spectrum disorders. Translational Psychiatry, 4, e343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haker, H., Kawohl, W., Herwig, U., & Rössler, W. (2013). Mirror neuron activity during contagious yawning—An fMRI study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 7, 2834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Helt, M. S., Eigsti, I. M., Snyder, P. J., & Fein, D. A. (2010). Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development. Child Development, 81, 16201631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helt, M. S., & Fein, D. A. (2016). Facial feedback and social input: Effects of laughter and enjoyment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 8394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 253261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 575583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hsu, C. T., Neufeld, J., & Chakrabarti, B. (2018). Reduced reward-related neural response to mimicry in individuals with autism. European Journal of Neuroscience, 47, 610618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudenko, W. J., Stone, W., & Bacharowski, J. A. (2009). Laughter differs in children with autism: An acoustic analysis of laughs produced by children with and without the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 13921400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 942951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Kaana-Kalman, R., & Goldman, S. (2008). Intermodal matching of emotional expressions in young children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 301310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, J. T., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding and cognitive empathy. Social Neuroscience, 1, 175183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Yirmiya, N. (1993). Focused and social attention of autistic children in interactions with familiar and unfamiliar adults: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 403414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J. L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron, 42, 335346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (2011). Repetitive movements and arousal. In Fein, D. (Ed.), The neuropsychology of autism (pp. 367394). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M., & Jordan, J. S. (2009). Embodied anticipation: A neurodevelopmental interpretation. Discourse Processes, 46, 103126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 358, 345360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knott, F., Lewis, C., & Williams, T. (1995). Sibling interaction of children with learning disabilities: A comparison of autism and Down's syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 36, 965976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Smith, A. (1997). Variables related to differences in standardized test outcomes for children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leighton, J., Bird, G., Charman, T., & Heyes, C. (2008). Weak imitative performance is not due to a functional “mirroring” deficit in adults with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46, 10411049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Likowski, K. U., Muhlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 10651072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2000). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.Google ScholarPubMed
Magnee, M. J. C. M., de Gelder, B., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2007). Facial electromyographic responses to emotional information from faces and voices in individuals with pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 11221130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markram, K., Rinaldi, T., & Markram, H. (2007). The intense world syndrome—An alternative hypothesis for autism. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 1, 7796. doi:10.3389/neuro.01/1.1.006.2007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McIntosh, D. (2006). Spontaneous facial mimicry, liking and emotional contagion. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 37, 3134.Google Scholar
McIntosh, D. N., Reichmann-Decker, A., Winkielman, P., & Wilbarger, J. L. (2006). When the social mirror breaks: Deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. Developmental Science, 9, 295302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, S., Zysset, D. Y., von Cramon, K., & Alter, K. (2005). Alter distinct fMRI responses to laughter, speech, and sounds along the human peri-sylvian cortex Brain Res. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 291306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moody, E. J., McIntosh, D. N., Mann, L. J., & Weisser, K. R. (2007). More than mere mimicry? The influence of emotion of rapid facial reactions to faces. Emotion, 7, 447457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Ric, P., & Krauth-Gruber, S. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norscia, I., & Palagi, E. (2011). Yawn contagion and empathy in Homo sapiens. PLOS ONE, 6, e28472. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028472CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autistic spectrum disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 310327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oberman, L. M., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2008). Modulation of mu suppression in children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: The mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 46, 15581565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2009). Slow echo: Facial EMG evidence for the delay of spontaneous, but not voluntary, emotional mimicry in children with autism spectrum disorders. Developmental Science, 12, 510520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfeifer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Maziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Mirroring other's emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children. Neuroimage, 39, 20762085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, K., Haist, F., Sedaghat, F., & Courchesne, E. (2004). The brain response to personally familiar faces in autism: Findings of fusiform activity and beyond. Brain, 127, 27032716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pierce, K., & Schreibman, L. (1997). Multiple peer use of pivotal response training to increase social behaviors of classmates with autism: Results from trained and untrained peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 157160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Platek, S. M., Myers, T. E., Critton, S. R., & Gallup, G. G. (2003). A left-hand advantage for self-description: The impact of schizotypal personality traits. Schizophrenia Research, 65, 147151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preston, S., & de Waal, F. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 120. doi:10.1017/S0140525X02000018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provine, R. R. (1986). Yawning as a stereotypical action pattern and releasing stimulus. Ethology, 71, 109122.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1989). Faces as releasers of contagious yawning: An approach to face detection using normal human subjects. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 211214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provine, R. R. (1996). Laughter. American Scientist, 84, 3845.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2005a). Yawning. American Scientist, 93, 532539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2005b). Contagious yawning and laughing: Everyday imitation- and mirror-like behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 142. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05390030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2012). Curious behavior: Yawning, laughing, hiccupping, and beyond. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Broken mirrors: A theory of autism. Scientific American, 295, 6269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., & Fabbri-Destro, M. (2010). Mirror neurons: From discovery to autism. Experimental Brain Research, 200, 223237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 37, 709715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford Binet Intelligence Scales (5th ed.). Rolling Hills, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Rutgers, A. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2004). Autism and attachment: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 11231134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scambler, D. J., Hepburn, S. L., Rutherford, M. D., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (2006). Emotional responsivity in children with autism, children with other developmental disabilities, and children with typical development. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 553563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fullbright, R., Anderson, R., Volkmar, A., … Gore, J. C. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome during face discrimination. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., Kikuchi, Y., Akechi, H., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., & Osanai, H. (2009). Brief report: Does eye contact induce contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 15981602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senju, A., Maeda, M., Kikuchi, Y., Hasegawa, T., Tojo, Y., & Osanai, H. (2007). Absence of contagious yawning in children with autism spectrum disorder. Biology Letters, 3, 706708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sigman, M., Dissanayake, C., Corona, R., & Espinosa, M. (2003). Social and cardiac responses of young children with autism. Autism, 7, 205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sims, T. B., Van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2012). How reward modulates mimicry: EMG evidence of greater facial mimicry of more rewarding happy faces. Psychophysiology, 49, 9981004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 8196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 11571162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, A. (2009). Emotional empathy in autism spectral conditions: Weak, intact, or heightened? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 17471748. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0799-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonnby-Borgstrom, M. (2002). Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 433443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Southgate, V., & Hamilton, A. F. (2008). Unbroken mirrors: Challenging a theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 225229. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stel, M., & Vonk, R. (2010). Mimicry in social interaction: Benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 311323. doi:10.1348/000712609X465424CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tickle-Degnen, L. (2006). Nonverbal behavior and its functions in the ecosystem of rapport. In Manusov, V. & Patterson, M. L. (Eds.), The Sage handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 381399). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Engeland, H., Roelofs, J. W., Verbaten, M. N., & Slangen, J. L. (1991). Abnormal electrodermal reactivity to novel visual stimuli in autistic children. Psychiatry Research, 38, 2738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of “imitation” in children. Developmental Science, 5, 141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waterhouse, L., Fein, D., & Modhal, C. (1996). Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. Psychological Review, 103, 457489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshimura, S., Sato, W., Uono, S., & Toichi, M. (2015). Impaired overt facial mimicry in response to dynamic facial expressions in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 13181328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed