Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-dnltx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T12:26:16.117Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal prediction of language emergence in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 April 2016

Sarah R. Edmunds*
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Lisa V. Ibañez
Affiliation:
University of Washington
Zachary Warren
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University
Daniel S. Messinger
Affiliation:
University of Miami
Wendy L. Stone
Affiliation:
University of Washington
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Sarah R. Edmunds, University of Washington, Box 351525, Seattle, WA 98195; E-mail: sre26@u.washington.edu.

Abstract

This study used a prospective longitudinal design to examine the early developmental pathways that underlie language growth in infants at high risk (n = 50) and low risk (n = 34) for autism spectrum disorder in the first 18 months of life. While motor imitation and responding to joint attention (RJA) have both been found to predict expressive language in children with autism spectrum disorder and those with typical development, the longitudinal relation between these capacities has not yet been identified. As hypothesized, results revealed that 15-month RJA mediated the association between 12-month motor imitation and 18-month expressive vocabulary, even after controlling for earlier levels of RJA and vocabulary. These results provide new information about the developmental sequencing of skills relevant to language growth that may inform future intervention efforts for children at risk for language delay or other developmental challenges.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahktar, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in early word learning. Child Development, 67, 635. doi:10.2307/1131837 Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed. Revised). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.) Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Anderson, D. K., Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., et al. (2007). Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 75, 594604.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1977). From gesture to the first word: On cognitive and social prerequisites. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (Eds.), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language (pp. 247–207). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4, Serial No. 255), 1174.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 91106.Google Scholar
CDC. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders among children 8 years old—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report Surveillance Summary, 63, 121.Google Scholar
Charman, T. (2004). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences, 358, 315324. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1199 Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, G. B., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 265285.Google Scholar
Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558577. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.558 Google Scholar
Constantino, J. N., Zhang, Y., Frazier, T., Abbacchi, A. M., & Law, P. (2010). Sibling recurrence and the genetic epidemiology of autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 13491356. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101470 Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Jones, E. J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K., Lowy, R., Faja, S., et al. (2012). Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young children with autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 11501159. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.018 Google Scholar
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., et al. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271283. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, C. E. F., Peter, M., Crowson, M., Markus, J., Yale, M., & Schwartz, H. (2002). Responding to joint attention and language development: A comparison of target locations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 715719.Google Scholar
Farrant, B. M., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, J. (2011). Socio-emotional engagement, joint attention, imitation, and conversation skill: Analysis in typical development and language specific impairment. First Language, 31, 2346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Egyed, K., & Király, I. (2007). On pedagogy. Developmental Science, 10, 139146. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00576.x Google Scholar
Goldfield, B. A., & Reznick, S. J. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: Rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language, 17, 171183.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10, 778785. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00636.x Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). “I beg your pardon?”: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language, 13, 455476. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900006826 Google Scholar
Goods, K. S., Ishijima, E., Chang, Y.-C., & Kasari, C. (2013). Preschool based JASPER intervention in minimally verbal children with autism: Pilot RCT. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 10501056. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1644-3 Google Scholar
Grønborg, T. K., Schendel, D. E., & Parner, E. T. (2013). Recurrence of autism spectrum disorders in full- and half-siblings and trends over time: A population-based cohort study. JAMA Pediatrics. Advance online publication. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.2259 Google Scholar
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P., & Hobson, J. A. (2008). Dissociable aspects of imitation: A study in autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 170185. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.007 Google Scholar
Hus, V., Pickles, A., Cook, E. H. Jr., Risi, S., & Lord, C. (2007). Using the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised to increase phenotypic homogeneity in genetic studies of autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 438448.Google Scholar
Ibañez, L. V, Grantz, C. J., & Messinger, D. S. (2013). The development of referential communication and autism symptomology in high-risk infants. Infancy, 18, 118. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00142.x.Google Scholar
Ingersoll, B. (2008). The effect of context on imitation skills in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 332340. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.08.003 Google Scholar
Ingersoll, B. (2012). Brief report: Effect of a focused imitation intervention on social functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 17681773. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1423-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingersoll, B., & Meyer, K. (2011). Examination of correlates of different imitative functions in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 10781085. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching reciprocal imitation skills to young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 487505. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0089-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. S. (2007). Imitation in infancy: The development of mimicry. Psychological Science, 18, 593599. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01945.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K. (2007). Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science, 10, 110120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00572.x Google Scholar
Landa, R. J., Gross, A. L., Stuart, E. A., & Faherty, A. (2013). Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism spectrum disorders: The first 3 years. Child Development, 84, 429442. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01870.x Google Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205223.Google Scholar
Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In Rice, M. L. & Warren, S. F. (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 729). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 14261438. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1 Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593614. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542 Google Scholar
Malesa, E., Foss-Feig, J., Yoder, P., Warren, Z., Walden, T., & Stone, W. (2012). Predicting language and social outcomes at age 5 for later-born siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1362361312444628 Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000222 Google Scholar
Maxwell, S. E., Cole, D., & Mitchell, M. (2011). Bias in cross-sectional analyses of longitudinal mediation: Partial and complete mediation under an autoregressive model. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46, 816841. doi:10.1080/00273171.2011.606716 Google Scholar
McDuffie, A., Turner, L., Stone, W., Yoder, P., Wolery, M., & Ulman, T. (2007). Developmental correlates of different types of motor imitation in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 401412.Google Scholar
McDuffie, A., Yoder, P., & Stone, W. (2005). Prelinguistic predictors of vocabulary in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 10801097.Google Scholar
McMurray, B. (2007). Defusing the childhood vocabulary explosion. Science, 317, 631. doi:10.1126/science.1144073 Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). The “like me” framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent. Acta Psychologica, 124, 2643. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.005 Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 335366). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 300308. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011 Google Scholar
Morales, M., Mundy, P., Delgado, C. E., Yale, M., Messinger, D., Neal, R., et al. (2000). Responding to joint attention across the 6- through 24-month age period and early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21, 283298.Google Scholar
Mullen, E. (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Circle Pines, MN: America Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., van Hecke, A. V., & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development, 78, 938954.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan, A., & Seibert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged Early Social Communication Scales (ESCS). Unpublished manuscript, University of Miami.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in young children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389401.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A. M. (2009). A parallel and distributed-processing model of joint attention, social cognition, and autism. Autism Research, 2, 221.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Guerini, C., Peze, A., & Rivet, C. (1999). The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In Nadel, J., & Butterworth, G. (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 209233). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A baby siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics, 128, e488e495.Google Scholar
Parladé, M. V., & Iverson, J. M. (2011). The interplay between language, gesture, and affect during communicative transition: A dynamic systems approach. Developmental Psychology, 47, 820833. doi:10.1037/a0021811 Google Scholar
Pinborough-Zimmerman, J., Satterfield, R., Miller, J., Bilder, D., Hossain, S., & McMahon, W. (2007). Communication disorders: Prevalence and comorbid intellectual disability, autism, and emotional/behavioral disorders. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 16, 359367. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2007/039) Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 185227. doi:10.1080/00273170701341316 Google Scholar
Presmanes, A. G., Walden, T. A., Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. J. (2007). Effects of different attentional cues on responding to joint attention in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 133144. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0338-0 Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 763781.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137162.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Vismara, L. A. (2008). Evidence-based comprehensive treatments for early autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 838. doi:10.1080/15374410701817808 Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview: Revised manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147177. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.7.2.147 Google Scholar
Selig, J. P., & Preacher, K. J. (2009). Mediation models for longitudinal data in developmental research. Research in Human Development, 6, 144164. doi:10.1080/15427600902911247 Google Scholar
Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1, Serial No. 256), 1114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44, 16911704.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Ousley, O. Y. (2000). Brief report: Screening Tool for Autism in Two-year-olds (STAT): Development and preliminary data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 607612.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. F., Turner, L. M., & Pozdol, S. L. (2004). Psychometric properties of the STAT for early autism screening. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 691701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, W. L., McMahon, C. R., & Henderson, L. M. (2008). Use of the Screening Tool for Autism in Two-year-olds (STAT) for children under 24 months: An exploratory study. Autism, 12, 557573.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., McMahon, C. R., Yoder, P. J., & Walden, T. A. (2007). Early social–communicative and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161, 384390. doi:10.1001/archpedi.161.4.384 Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What's the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 475485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5, 341361.Google Scholar
Sullivan, M., Finelli, J., Marvin, A., Garrett-Mayer, E., Bauman, M., & Landa, R. (2007). Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorder: A prospective study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 3748. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0335-3 Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103130). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 9931005.Google Scholar
Turner, L. M., & Stone, W. L. (2007). Variability in outcome for children with an ASD diagnosis at age 2. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 48, 793802. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01744.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uzgiris, I. (1981). Two functions of imitation in infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 112.Google Scholar
Walton, K. M., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2013). Expressive and receptive fast-mapping in children with autism spectrum disorders and typical development: The influence of orienting cues. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 687698. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.02.012 Google Scholar
Wu, C., & Chiang, C. (2014). The developmental sequence of social communicative skills in young children with autism: A longitudinal study. Autism, 18, 385392. doi:10.1177/1362361313479832 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, G. S., Rogers, S. J., Hutman, T., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Ozonoff, S. (2011). Imitation from 12 to 24 months in autism and typical development: A longitudinal Rasch analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47, 15651578.Google Scholar
Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143152. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001 Google Scholar