Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T15:17:46.566Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal elaborative reminiscing mediates the effect of child maltreatment on behavioral and physiological functioning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 November 2015

Kristin Valentino*
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame
Leah C. Hibel
Affiliation:
University of California, Davis
E. Mark Cummings
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame
Amy K. Nuttall
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame
Michelle Comas
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame
Christina G. McDonnell
Affiliation:
University of Notre Dame
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Kristin Valentino, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, 128 Haggar Hall, Notre Dame, IN 46556; E-mail: kristin.valentino@nd.edu.

Abstract

Theoretical and empirical evidence suggest that the way in which parents discuss everyday emotional experiences with their young children (i.e., elaborative reminiscing) has significant implications for child cognitive and socioemotional functioning, and that maltreating parents have a particularly difficult time in engaging in this type of dialogue. This dyadic interactional exchange, therefore, has the potential to be an important process variable linking child maltreatment to developmental outcomes at multiple levels of analysis. The current investigation evaluated the role of maternal elaborative reminiscing in associations between maltreatment and child cognitive, emotional, and physiological functioning. Participants included 43 maltreated and 49 nonmaltreated children (aged 3–6) and their mothers. Dyads participated in a joint reminiscing task about four past emotional events, and children participated in assessments of receptive language and emotion knowledge. Child salivary cortisol was also collected from children three times a day (waking, midday, and bedtime) on 2 consecutive days to assess daily levels and diurnal decline. Results indicated that maltreating mothers engaged in significantly less elaborative reminiscing than did nonmaltreating mothers. Maternal elaborative reminiscing mediated associations between child maltreatment and child receptive language and child emotion knowledge. In addition, there was support for an indirect pathway between child maltreatment and child cortisol diurnal decline through maternal elaborative reminiscing. Directions for future research are discussed, and potential clinical implications are addressed.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alessandri, S. (1992). Mother–child interactional correlates of maltreated and nonmaltreated children's play behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 257270.Google Scholar
Anda, R., Felitti, V., Bremner, J., Walker, J., Whitfield, C., & Perry, B. (2006). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood—A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256, 174186.Google Scholar
Barnett, D., Ganiban, J., & Cicchetti, D. (1999). Maltreatment, negative expressivity, and the development of Type D attachments from 12- to 24-months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 97118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The interference between policy and research. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), Child abuse, child development and social policy (pp. 773). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Bohanek, J. G., Marin, K. A., & Fivush, R. (2008). Family narratives, self, and gender in early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 28, 153176.Google Scholar
Boland, A. M., Haden, C. A., & Ornstein, P. A. (2003). Boosting children's memory by training mothers in the use of an elaborative conversational style as an event unfolds. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 3965.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brame, B., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Developmental trajectories of physical aggression from school entry to late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 503512.Google Scholar
Bruce, J., Fisher, P. A., Pears, K. C., & Levine, S. (2009). Morning cortisol levels in preschool-aged foster children: Differential effects of maltreatment type. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 1423.Google Scholar
Burns, B., Phillips, S., Wagner, H., Barth, R., Kolko, D., & Campbell, Y. (2004). Mental health need and access to mental health services by youths involved with child welfare: A national survey. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 960970.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D. (2009). Regulatory competence and early disruptive behavior problems: The role of physiological regulation. In Olson, S. L. & Sameroff, A. (Eds.), Biopsychosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavioral problems (pp. 86115). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, L. L., Carvalho, J. P., Tyrka, A. R., Wier, L. M., Mello, A. F., Mello, M. F., et al. (2007). Decreased adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol responses to stress in healthy adults reporting significant child maltreatment. Biological Psychiatry, 62, 10801087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1990). The organization and coherence of socioemotional, cognitive, and representational development: Illustrations through a developmental psychopathology perspective on Downs syndrome and child maltreatment. In Thompson, R. (Ed.), Socioemotional development: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1988 (pp. 259367). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2001). Diverse patterns of neuroendocrine activity in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 677693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2015). A multilevel perspective on child maltreatment. In Lamb, M. & Garcia Coll, C. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Vol. 3. Socioemotional process (7th ed., pp. 513563). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Manly, J. T. (2003). Maternal Maltreatment Classification Interview. Unpublished manuscript, Mt. Hope Family Center.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2006). An ecological-transactional perspective on child maltreatment: Failure of the average expectable environment and its influence on child development. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 129201). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Valentino, K. (2007). Toward the application of a multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective to research in development and psychopathology. In Masten, A. (Ed.), The Minnesota symposia on child development: Multilevel dynamics in developmental psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 243284). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coster, W. J., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Research on the communicative development of maltreated children: Clinical implications. Topics in Language Disorders, 13, 2538.Google Scholar
Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1990). Minor parenting stresses with young children. Child Development, 61, 16281637.Google Scholar
Crockett, E. E., Holmes, B. M., Granger, D. A., & Lyons-Ruth, K. (2013). Maternal disrupted communication during face-to-face interaction at 4 months: Relation to maternal and infant cortisol among at-risk families. Infancy, 18, 11111134.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denham, S. A., & Burton, R. (2003). Social and emotional prevention and intervention programming for preschoolers. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Denham, S. A., & Couchoud, E.A. (1990). Young preschooler's understanding of emotions. Child Study Journal, 20, 171192.Google Scholar
Dixon, L., Browne, K., & Hamilton-Giachritsis, C. (2005). Risk factors of parents abused as children: A mediational analysis of the intergenerational continuity of child maltreatment (part I). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 46, 4757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Edwards, A., Shipman, K., & Brown, A. (2005). The socialization of emotional understanding: A comparison of neglectful and nonneglectful mothers and their children. Child Maltreatment, 10, 293304.Google Scholar
Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Attachment and early maltreatment. Child Development, 52, 4452.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science, 7, 88102.Google Scholar
Essex, M. J., Shirtcliff, E. A., Burka, L. R., Ruttle, P. L., Klein, M. H., Slattery, M. J., et al. (2011). Influence of early life stress on later hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning and its covariation with mental health symptoms: A study of the allostatic process from childhood into adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 10391058.Google Scholar
Finegood, E., Blair, D., Granger, D., Hibel, L. C., Mills-Koonce, W. R., & Investigators, TFLP. (2015). Maternal salivary cortisol and cumulative risk predict parenting behavior in the postpartum period. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (1993). Emotional content of parent-child conversations about the past. In Nelson, C. A. (Ed.), The Minnesota symposia in child psychology: Memory and affect in development (Vol. 26, pp. 3978). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., Brotman, M., Buckner, J. P., & Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles, 42, 233253.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., & Fromhoff, F. (1988). Style and structure in mother child conversations about the past. Discourse Processes, 11, 337355.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., Haden, C., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77, 15681588.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., & Sales, J. (2006). Coping, attachment, and mother-child narratives of stressful events. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 52, 125150.Google Scholar
Fivush, R., & Vasudeva, A. (2002). Remembering to relate: Socioemotional correlates of mother-child reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 7390.Google Scholar
Gonzalez, A., Jenkins, J. M., Steiner, M., & Fleming, A. S. (2009). The relation between early life adversity, cortisol awakening response and diurnal salivary cortisol levels in postpartum women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 7686. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.012 Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the emotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 243268.Google Scholar
Gunnar, M., Fisch, R., & Malone, S. (1984). The effects of a pacifying stimulus on behavioral and adrenocortical responses to circumcision in the newborn. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23, 3438.Google Scholar
Harley, K., & Reese, E. (1999). Origins of autobiographical memory. Developmental Psychology, 35, 13381348.Google Scholar
Hart, T. R., & Risley, B. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American Chlidren. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hooven, C., Gottman, J. M., & Katz, L. F. (1995). Parental meta-emotion structure predicts marital quality and children's socioemotional outcomes. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 229264.Google Scholar
Huber, T. J., Issa, K., Schik, G., & Wolf, O. T. (2006). The cortisol awakening response in blunted in psychotherapy inpatients suffering from depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 900904.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343.Google Scholar
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., & Getzler-Yosef, R. (2004). Mothers who were severely abused during childhood and their children talk about emotions: Co-construction of narratives in light of maternal trauma. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 300317.Google Scholar
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., & Getzler-Yosef, R. (2008). Shaping children's internal working models through mother–child dialogues: The importance of resolving past maternal trauma. Attachment Human Development, 10, 465483.Google Scholar
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., Haimovich, Z., & Etzion-Carasso, A. (2003). Dialogues of seven-year-olds with their mothers about emotional events: Development of typology. In Emde, R. N., Wolf, D. P., & Oppenheim, D. (Eds.), Revealing the inner worlds of young children: The MacArthur Story Stem Battery and Parent-Child Narratives (pp. 338354). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004). Mother–child discourse surrounding a child's past behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 50, 159181.Google Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Bronfman, E., & Parsons (1999). Maternal frightened, frightening, or atypical behavior and disorganized infant attachment patterns. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(3), 6796.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99128.Google Scholar
Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. R., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Dimensions of child maltreatment and children's adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 759782.Google Scholar
Martorell, G. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2006). Maternal variations in stress reactivity: Implications for harsh parenting practices with very young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 641.Google Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, 3344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998–2014). Mplus user's guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles: Author.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 18.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486.Google Scholar
Parke, R. D., Cassidy, J., Burks, V. M., Carson, J. L., & Boyum, L. (1992). Familial contributions to peer competence among young children: The role of interactive and affective processes. In Parke, R. D. & Ladd, G. W. (Eds.), Family–peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 107134). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pears, K., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Developmental, cognitive, and neuropsychological functioning in preschool-aged foster children: Associations with prior maltreatment and placement history. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 112122.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26, 49.Google Scholar
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 916931.Google Scholar
Reese, E. (1995). Predicting children's literacy from mother-child conversations. Cognitive Development, 10, 381405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., & Newcombe, R. (2007). Training mothers in elaborative reminiscing enhances children's autobiographical memory and narrative. Child Development, 78, 11531170.Google Scholar
Rogosch, F., Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (1995). The role of child maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive and affective processing abilities and later peer relationship problems. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 591609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruttle, P. L., Shirtcliff, E. A., Serbin, L. A., Ben-Dat Fisher, D., Stack, D. M., & Schwartzman, A. E. (2011). Disentangling psychobiological mechanisms underlying internalizing and externalizing behaviors in youth: Longitudinal and concurrent associations with cortisol. Hormones and Behavior, 59, 123132.Google Scholar
Sales, J., & Fivush, R. (2005). Social and emotional functions of mother-child reminiscing about stressful events. Social Cognition, 23, 7090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sales, J., Fivush, R., & Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminiscing about positive and negative events. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 185209.Google Scholar
Salmon, K., Dadds, M., Allen, J., & Hawes, D. (2009). Can emotional language skills be taught during parent training for conduct problem children? Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40, 485498.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. E. (2009). The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Shipman, K. L., Schneider, R., Fitzgerald, M. M., Sims, C., Swisher, L., & Edwards, A. (2007). Maternal emotion socialization in maltreating and non-maltreating families: Implications for children's emotion regulation. Social Development, 16, 268285.Google Scholar
Shipman, K. L., & Zeman, J. (1999). Emotional understanding: A comparison of physically maltreating and nonmaltreating mother-child dyads. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 407417.Google Scholar
Smyth, N., Hucklebridge, F., Thorn, L., Evan, P., & Clow, A. (2013). Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in social science research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 605625.Google Scholar
Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Iig, U., Maier, U., & Ackermann, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 425437.Google Scholar
Sparks, A., & Reese, E. (2013). From reminiscing to reading: Home contributions to children's developing language and literacy in low-income families. First Language, 33, 89109.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Early relationships and the development of children. Infant Mental Health Journal, 21, 6774.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L., Carlson, E., Levy, A., & Egeland, B. (1999). Implications of attachment theory for developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 113.Google Scholar
Stacks, A., Beeghly, M., Partridge, T., & Dexter, C. (2011). Effects of placement type on the language developmental trajectories of maltreated children from infancy to early childhood. Child Maltreatment, 16, 287299.Google Scholar
Stahmer, A. C., Hurlburt, M., Horwitz, S. M., Landsverk, J., Zhang, J., & Leslie, L. K. (2009). Associations between intensity child welfare involvement and child development among young children in child welfare. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 598611.Google Scholar
Stock, C. D., & Fisher, P. A. (2006). Language delays among foster children: Implications for policy and practice. Child Welfare, 85, 445461.Google Scholar
Sullivan, M. W., Bennett, D. S., Carpenter, K., & Lewis, M. (2008). Emotion knowledge in young neglected children. Child Maltreatment, 13, 301306.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb, M. J. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year olds: Contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Development, 75, 18061820.Google Scholar
Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2006) Child maltreatment and the developing HPA axis. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 632639.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. (2006). Conversation and developing understanding: Introduction to the Special Issue. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 52, 116.Google Scholar
Toth, S. L., Gravener-Davis, J. A., Guild, D. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2013). Relational interventions for child maltreatment: Past, present, and future perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 16011617.Google Scholar
US Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families. (2015). Child maltreatment 2013. Retrieved from http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/resource/child-maltreatment-2013 Google Scholar
Valentino, K., Cicchetti, D., Toth, S., & Rogosch, F. (2006). Mother-child play and emerging social behaviors among infants from maltreating families. Developmental Psychology, 42, 474485.Google Scholar
Valentino, K., Comas, M., Nuttall, A. K., & Thomas, T. (2013). Training maltreating parents in elaborative and emotion-rich reminiscing with their preschool-aged children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37, 585595.Google Scholar
Valentino, K., Nuttall, A. K., Comas, M., McDonnell, C. G., Piper, B., Thomas, T., et al. (2014). Mother-child reminiscing and autobiographical memory specificity among preschool-aged children. Developmental Psychology, 50, 11971207.Google Scholar
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., Dadds, M., & Allen, J. (2009). Training mothers in emotion-rich elaborative reminiscing: Facilitating children's autobiographical memory and emotion knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10, 162187.Google Scholar
van der Vegt, E. J. M., van der Ende, J., Kirschbaum, C., Verhulst, F. C., & Teimerier, H. (2009) Early neglect and abuse predict diurnal cortisol patterns in adults: A study of international adoptees. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 660669.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225249.Google Scholar
Wareham, P., & Salmon, K. (2006). Mother–child reminiscing about everyday experiences: Implications for psychological interventions in the preschool years. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 535554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule—Expanded form. Ames, IA: University of Iowa Pres.Google Scholar
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar