Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T01:38:46.526Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Imitating actions on objects in early-onset and regressive autism: Effects and implications of task characteristics on performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2010

Sally J. Rogers*
Affiliation:
University of California Davis
Gregory S. Young
Affiliation:
University of California Davis
Ian Cook
Affiliation:
University of California Davis
Angelo Giolzetti
Affiliation:
University of California Davis
Sally Ozonoff
Affiliation:
University of California Davis
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Sally J. Rogers, 2825 50th Street, Sacramento, CA 95817; E-mail: sally.rogers@ucdmc.ucdavis.edu.

Abstract

This study was designed to examine the nature of object imitation performance in early autism. We hypothesized that imitation would be relatively preserved when behaviors on objects resulted in salient instrumental effects. We designed tasks in which, in one condition, the motor action resulted in a salient, meaningful effect on an object, whereas in the other condition, the same action resulted in a less salient effect because of differing object characteristics. The motor aspects of the tasks did not vary across conditions. Four participant groups of 2- to 5-year-olds were examined: 17 children with early-onset autism, 24 children with regressive onset autism, 22 children with developmental delays, and 22 children with typical development. Groups were matched on nonverbal skills, and differences in verbal development were examined as a moderator of imitative ability. Results revealed an interaction of group by condition, with the combined autism group failing more tasks than the combined comparison groups, and failing more tasks in the less salient condition than in the more salient condition, as hypothesized. Analyses of autism subgroups revealed these effects were primarily because of the regression onset group. Accuracy of motor performance was examined by analyzing errors. Among children passing imitative acts, there were no group differences and no condition effects in the number, type, or pattern of performance errors. Among children passing the tasks, the group with autism did not demonstrate more emulation errors (imitating the goal but not the means) than other groups. There was no evidence that either motor or attentional aspects of the tasks contributed to the poorer imitative performance of the children with autism.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amaral, D. G., Bauman, M. D., & Mills Schumann, C. (2003). The amygdala and autism: Implications from non-human primate studies. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 2, 295302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bachevalier, J. (1994). Medial temporal lobe structures and autism: A review of clinical and experimental findings. Neuropsychologica, 6, 627648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1906). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ballinger, G. A. (2004). Using generalized estimating equations for longitudinal data analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennetto, L. (1999). A componential approach to imitation and movement deficits in autism. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 819.Google Scholar
Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Murias, M., & Webb, S. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64, 228237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27, 687708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 2830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Defining the early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and responses to emotions. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeMyer, M. K., Alpern, G. D., Barton, S., DeMyer, W. E., Churchill, D. W., Hingtgen, J. N., et al. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early schizophrenic, and nonpsychotic subnormal children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 264287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Kiraly, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, W. A., Thorsen, K. L., Osann, K., & Spence, M. A. (2007). Use of home videotapes to confirm parental reports of regression in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 11361146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, J. A., & Hobson, R. P. (2007). Identification: The missing link between joint attention and imitation? Development and Psychopathology, 19, 411431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1995). Apprehending attitudes and actions: Separable abilities in early development? Development and Psychopathology, 7, 171182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 649660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University.Google Scholar
Huang, C. T., Heyes, C., & Charman, T. (2002). Infants' behavioral reenactment of “Failed Attempts”: Exploring the roles of emulation learning, stimulus enhancement, and understanding of intentions. Developmental Psychology, 38, 840855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 632637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding others: Imitation, language, and empathy. In Hurley, S. & Chater, N. (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From mirror neurons to memes—Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ingersoll, B., Schreibman, L., & Tran, Q. H. (2003). Effect of sensory feedback on immediate object imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 673683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, K., & Marcus, J. (1981). Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda. Developmental Psychology, 17, 258265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liang, K. Y., & Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73, 1322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—WPS Edition. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Luyster, R., Richler, J., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., et al. (2005). Early regression in social communication in autism spectrum disorders: A CPEA study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 311336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cesari, A., Pecini, C., Apicella, F., & Stern, D. (2006). A view to regressive autism through home movies. Is early development really normal? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 113, 6872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. F., & Ritz, E. G. (1984). Patterns of gestural, vocal, and verbal imitation performance in infancy. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 30, 369392.Google Scholar
McCabe, M. A., & Uzgiris, I. C. (1983). Effects of model and action on imitation in infancy. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 29, 6982.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Understanding other minds (pp. 335366). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10, 126134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, J., & Hobson, R. P. (2004). Orientation in relation to self and other: The case of autism. Interaction Studies, 5, 221244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miles, J. H., Takahashi, T. N., Bagby, S., Sahota, P. K., Vaslow, D. F., Wang, C. H., et al. (2005). Essential versus complex autism: Definition of fundamental prognostic subtypes. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 135, 171180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Janciewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., & Denckla, M. B. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 314326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Cranston, RI: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., & Neal, R. A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In Glidden, L. M. (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (pp. 139168). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationships, conceptualization, and gestural imitation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 4562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 6279.Google ScholarPubMed
Richler, J., Luyster, R., Risi, S., Hsu, W. L., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., et al. (2006). Is there a regressive “phenotype” of autism spectrum disorder associated with the measles–mumps–rubella vaccine? A CPEA study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 299316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In Nadel, J. & Butterworth, G. (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 254283). Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J. (2004). Developmental regression in autism spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10, 139143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., Bennetto, L., McEvoy, R., & Pennington, B. F. (1996). Imitation and pantomime in high functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Child Development, 67, 20602073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., & DiLalla, D. (1990). Age of symptom onset in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 863872.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 763781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Williams, J. H. G. (2006). Imitation in autism: Findings and controversies. In Rogers, S. J. & Williams, J. H. G. (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 277309). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rumiati, R. I., Weiss, P. H., Tessari, A., Assmus, A., Zilles, K., Herzog, H., et al. (2005). Common and differential neural mechanisms supporting imitation of meaningful and meaningless actions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 14201431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1998). Gesture imitation in autism I: Nonsymbolic postures and sequences. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 747770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., & Littleford, C. D. (1997). Motor imitation in young children with autism: What's the object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 475485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1993). Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Development, 64, 16881705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, E., Dawson, G., Munson, J., & Osterling, J. (2005). Variation in early developmental course in autism and its relation with behavioral outcome at 3–4 years of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 337350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 157162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and “mirror neuron” functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 44, 610621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers' understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42, 723731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed