Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-995ml Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T04:46:28.545Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Translational research on early language development: Current challenges and future directions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2006

MARJORIE BEEGHLY
Affiliation:
Harvard Medical School and Children's Hospital, Boston

Abstract

There is a pressing need for the early and accurate identification of young children at risk for language and other developmental disabilities and the provision of timely, age-appropriate intervention, as mandated by Part C of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. Research has shown that early intervention is effective for many language impaired children in different etiological groups, and can reduce the functional impact of persistent disorders on children and their families. Yet, the accurate identification of infants and toddlers at risk for language impairment remains elusive, especially for late-talking children without obvious genetic or neurological conditions. In this paper, the need for translational research on basic processes in early language development in typical and atypical populations and the contextual factors that affect them are discussed, along with current challenges and future directions for its successful implementation. Implications of this research for clinical evidence-based practice are also considered.Partial support during the writing of this paper was provided by NIH Grant RO1HD044068. I am grateful to Dante Cicchetti for his thoughtful feedback on a prior version of this paper.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbeduto, L., & Hagerman, R. J. (1997). Language and communication in fragile X syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 313322.Google Scholar
Abbeduto, L., & Murphy, M. M. (2004). Language, social cognition, maladaptive behavior, and communication in Down syndrome and fragile X syndrome. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 7797). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Children with Disabilities. (2001). Developmental surveillance and screening of infants and young children. Pediatrics, 108, 192196.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139148.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839843.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (2003). Explaining and interpreting deficits in language development across clinical groups: Where do we go from here? Brain and Language, 88, 248253.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Beeghly, M., & McNew, S. (1982). Social bases of language development: A reassessment. In H. Reese & L. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 16, pp. 775). New York: Academic Press.
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., Marchman, V., Thal, D. J., Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., et al. (1994). Developmental and stylistic variation in the composition of early vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 21, 85124.Google Scholar
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247258.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M. (1997). Emergence of symbolic play: Perspectives from typical and atypical development. In J. Burack, R. Hodapp, & E. Zigler (Eds.), Handbook of mental retardation and development (pp. 240289). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1994). Child maltreatment, attachment, and the self system: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in toddlers at high social risk. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 530.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Talking about self and other: Emergence of an internal state lexicon in young children with Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 729748.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Martin, B., Rose-Jacobs, R., Cabral, H., Heeren, T., Augustyn, M., et al. (2006). Prenatal cocaine exposure and children's language functioning at 6 and 9.5 years: Moderating effects of child age, birthweight, and gender. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 98115.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Perry, B. W., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Structural and affective dimensions of play development in young children with Down syndrome. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 257277.Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Weiss-Perry, B., & Cicchetti, D. (1990). Beyond sensorimotor functioning: Early communicative and play development of children with Down syndrome. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 329368). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bellugi, U., Wang, P. P., & Jernigan, T. L. (1994). Williams syndrome: An unusual neuropsychological profile. In S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders (pp. 2356). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Berko Gleason, J. (2005). The development of language (6th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Billingsley-Marshall, R. L., Simos, P. G., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2004). Reliability and validity of functional neuroimaging techniques for identifying language—Critical areas in children and adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 541563.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M. (1994). Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 507550.Google Scholar
Bishop, D. V. M., Chan, J., Adams, C., Hartley, J., & Weir, F. (2000). Conversational responsiveness in specific language impairment: Evidence of disproportionate pragmatic difficulties in a subset of children. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 177199.Google Scholar
Blacher, J., Kraemer, B., & Schalow, M. (2003). Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism: Research concerns and emerging foci. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 16, 535542.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25, 367393.Google Scholar
Briscoe, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Marlow, N. (1998). Short-term memory and language outcomes after extreme prematurity at birth. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 654666.Google Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., Keller, T., Cherkassky, V., Roth, J. K., & Minshew, N. (2001). Dynamic cortical systems subserving cognition: fMRI studies with typical and atypical individuals. In J. L. McClelland & R. Siegler (Eds.), Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives (pp. 353383). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Catts, H. W. (1993). The relationship between speech–language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948958.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. S. (2003). Language and communication in individuals with Down syndrome. In L. Abbeduto (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 27, pp. 134). New York: Academic Press.
Chapman, R. S., & Hesketh, L. (2001). Language, cognition, and short-term memory in individuals with Down syndrome. Down Syndrome: Research and Practice, 7, 17.Google Scholar
Charman, T. (1997). The relationship between joint attention and pretend play in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 116.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481498.Google Scholar
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213236.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 17.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Beeghly, M. (1990). An organizational approach to the study of Down syndrome: Contributions to an integrative theory of development. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 2962). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cicchetti, D., Beeghly, M., Carlson, V., Coster, W., Gersten, M., Rieder, C., et al. (1991). Development and psychopathology: Lessons from the study of maltreated children. In D. P. Keating & H. Rosen (Eds.), Constructivist perspectives on developmental psychopathology and atypical development. The Jean Piaget symposium series (pp. 69102). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cicchetti, D., Beeghly, M., & Weiss-Perry, B. (1994). Symbolic development in children with Down syndrome and in children with autism: An organizational, developmental psychopathology perspective. In A. Slade & D. P. Wolf (Eds.), Children at play: Clinical and Developmental approaches to meaning and representation (pp. 206237). New York: Oxford University Press.
Cielinski, K. L., Vaughn, B. E., Seifer, R., & Contreras, J. (1995). Relations among sustained engagement during play, quality of play, and mother–child interaction in samples of children with Down syndrome and normally developing toddlers. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 163176.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H., & Almazan, M. (1998). Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167198.Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J., Davine, M., Horodezky, N., Lipsett, L., & Isaacson, L. (1993). Unsuspected language impairment in psychiatrically disturbed children: Prevalence and language and behavioral characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 1103.Google Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 741748.Google Scholar
Coster, W., Gersten, M., Beeghly, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 10201029.Google Scholar
Dale, P., Greenberg, M., & Crnic, K. A. (1987). The multiple determinants of symbolic development: Evidence from preterm children. New Directions for Child Development, 36, 6986.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: I. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 544560.Google Scholar
Dawson, G., & Castelloe, P. (1992). Autism. In D. E. Walker & M. C. Roberts (Eds.), Handbook of child clinical psychology (2nd ed., pp. 375397). New York: Wiley.
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394412.Google Scholar
Dunham, P., & Dunham, F. (1992). Lexical development during middle infancy: A mutually driven infant–caregiver process. Developmental Psychology, 28, 414420.Google Scholar
Dunst, C. J. (2000). Revisiting “rethinking early intervention.” Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20, 95104.Google Scholar
Dykens, E., Hodapp, R., & Finucane, B. M. (Eds.). (2000). Genetics and mental retardation syndromes: A new look at behavior and interventions. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Dykens, E. M. (2003). The Williams syndrome behavioral phenotype: The “whole person” is missing. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 16, 523528.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science, 7, 88102.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Feldman, H. M. (2005). Evaluation and management of language and speech disorders in preschool children. Pediatrics in Review, 26, 131141.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Series no. 242).Google Scholar
Fowler, A. E. (1990). Language abilities in children with Down syndrome: Evidence for a specific syntactic delay. In D. Cicchetti & M. Beeghly (Eds.), Children with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective (pp. 302328). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Goldfield, B. A., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Individual differences: Implications for the study of language acquisition. In J. Berko Gleason (Ed.), The development of language (6th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Grizzle, K. L., & Simms, M. D. (2005). Early language development and language learning disabilities. Pediatrics in Review, 26, 274283.Google Scholar
Guralnick, M. J. (1997). The effectiveness of early intervention. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Happe, F. (1995). Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Happe, F., & Frith, U. (1996). The neuropsychology of autism. Brain, 119, 13771400.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Hauser-Cram, P., Warfield, M. E., Shonkoff, J. P., & Krauss, M. W. (2001). Children with disabilities: A longitudinal study of child development and parent well-being. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 1131.Google Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. (1997). Direct and indirect behavioral effects of different genetic disorders on mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 102, 6780.Google Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. (2004). Studying interactions, reactions, and perceptions: Can genetic disorders serve as behavioral proxies? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 2934.Google Scholar
Joseph, R. M. (1999). Neuropsychological frameworks for understanding autism. International Review of Psychiatry, 11, 309325.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2001). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. In M. Tomasello & E. Bates (Eds.), Language development: The essential readings (pp. 331350). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Thomas, M. (2003). What can developmental disorders tell us about the neurocomputational constraints that shape development? The case of Williams syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 969990.Google Scholar
Kennedy, M. D., Sheridan, M. K., Radlinsky, S. H., & Beeghly, M. (1991). Play–language relationships in young children with development delays: Implications on assessment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 112122.Google Scholar
Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsodl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., et al. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5, 233246.Google Scholar
Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., & Smith, K. E. (2002). Individual differences in the development of social communication competency in very low birthweight children. In D. L. Molfese & V. J. Molfese (Eds.), Developmental variations in learning: Applications to social, executive function, language, and reading skills (pp. 81112). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., & Swank, P. R. (1997). Predicting cognitive–language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33, 10401053.Google Scholar
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of Langauge and Communication Disorders, 35, 165188.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 729). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (1994). Autism and pervasive development disorders. In E. Taylor (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (Vol. 3, pp. 569593). Oxford: Blackwell.
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543562.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Saccuman, C., & Wulfeck, B. (2003). Productive use of the English past tense in children with focal brain injury and specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 88, 202214.Google Scholar
Massey, A. E. (1996). Cultural effects on language: Implications for assessing African American children. In A. Kamhi & K. Pollock (Eds.), Communication development and disorders in African American children: Research, assessment, and intervention (pp. 285306). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
McCardle, P., Cooper, J., & Freund, L. (2005). Language and genetics: Needs and opportunities. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 129135.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (2003). Williams syndrome: 15 years of psychological research. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 112.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Klein-Tasman, B. (2004). Methodological issues in group matching designs: Alpha levels for control variable comparisons and measurement characteristics of control and target variables. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 717.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Klein-Tasman, B., Bertrand, J., Kwitny, S., Abbelbaum, L. G., et al. (2003). Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome during triadic interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 243268.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (2000). Expressive vocabulary ability of toddlers with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome: A comparison. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 111126.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. F. (2005). Designing measures for profiling and genotype/phenotype studies of individuals with genetic syndromes or developmental language disorders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 4164.Google Scholar
Messer, D. J. (1994). The development of communication: From social interaction to language. Chicester: Wiley.
Miller, J. (1990). The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Morisset, C. E., Barnard, K. E., & Booth, C. L. (1995). Toddlers' language development: Sex differences within social risk. Developmental Psychology, 31, 851865.Google Scholar
Morris, C. A. (2004). Genotype-phenotype correlations: Lessons from Williams syndrome research. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 355369). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Muller, R. A. (2005). Neurocognitive studies of language impairments: The bottom-up approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 6578.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115128.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., & Kasari, C. (1993). The theory of mind and joint-attention deficits in autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mundy, P., Sigman, M. D., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention, developmental level, and symptom presentation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389401.Google Scholar
Murray, A. D., & Yingling, J. L. (2000). Competence in language at 24 months: Relations with attachment security and home stimulation. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 161, 133140.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain–behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 499520.Google Scholar
Newschaffer, C. J., Falb, M. D., & Gurney, J. G. (2005). National autism prevalence trends from United States special education data. Pediatrics, 115, 277282.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Reseach Network. (1999). Chronicity of maternal depressive symptoms, maternal sensitivity, and child functioning at 36 months. Developmental Psychology, 35, 12971310.Google Scholar
Olds, D. L. (2006). The nurse-based partnership: An evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 27, 525.Google Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763782.Google Scholar
Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of slow expressive language development. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 5, 521.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2005). Electrophysiology in the study of developmental language impairments: Prospects and challenges for a top-down approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 7996.Google Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicap. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 375383.Google Scholar
Pressman, L. J., Pipp-Siegel, S., Yoshinaga-Ito, C., & Deas, A. (1999). Maternal sensitivity predicts language gain in preschool children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 4, 294304.Google Scholar
Ramey, C. T., Campbell, F. A., & Ramey, S. L. (1999). Early intervention: Successful pathways to improving intellectual development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 16, 385392.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1991). Once more with feeling: Affect and language in atypical populations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 367391.Google Scholar
Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229247.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes at age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 45, 360371.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L. (2003). A unified model of specific and general language delay: Grammatical tense as a clinical marker of unexpected variation. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Toward a definition of specific language impairment (pp. 6395). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rice, M. L. (2004). Growth models of developmental language disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 207240). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rice, M. L., Hadley, P. A., & Alexander, A. (1993). Social biases toward children with speech and language impairments: A correlative causal model of language limitations. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 443471.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Warren, S. F. (2004). Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rice, M. L., & Warren, S. F. (2005a). Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rice, M. L., & Warren, S. F. (2005b). Moving toward a unified effort to understand the nature and causes of language disorders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 36.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Warren, S. F., & Betz, S. K. (2005). Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 727.Google Scholar
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., Marquis, J., & Hershberger, S. (2000). Acquisition of irregular past tense by children with SLI. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 11261145.Google Scholar
Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. R. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children's language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 345359.Google Scholar
Robinson, B. F., Mervis, C. B., & Robinson, B. W. (2003). The roles of verbal short-term memory and working memory in the acquisition of grammar by children with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 1231.Google Scholar
Roeyers, H., VanOost, P., & Bothuyne, S. (1998). Immediate imitation and joint attention in young children with autism. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 441450.Google Scholar
Rozien, N. J. (1997). Down syndrome. In M. L. Batshaw (Ed.), Children with disabilities (pp. 361376). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (1990). Transactional regulation and early intervention. In S. J. Meisels & J. P. Shonkoff (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood intervention (pp. 119149). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Shore, C. M. (1995). Individual differences in language development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Sigman, M. (1994). What are the core deficits in autism? In S. H. Broman & J. Grafman (Eds.), Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders (pp. 139158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sigman, M. (2005). Improvement in cognitive and language skills from preschool to adolescence in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 1523.Google Scholar
Singer, N. G., Bellugi, U., Bates, E., Jones, W., & Rossen, M. (1994). Contrasting profiles of language development in children with Williams and Down syndromes, Project in cognitive and neural development technical report. La Jolla, CA: University of California, San Diego.
Smith, S. D., & Morris, C. A. (2005). Planning studies of etiology. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 97110.Google Scholar
Spiker, D., Boyce, G. C., & Boyce, L. K. (2002). Parent–child interactions when young children have disabilities. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 25, 3570.Google Scholar
Spinath, F., Price, T., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2004). The genetic and environmental origins of language disability and ability. Child Development, 75, 445454.Google Scholar
Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year-old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 677696.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004a). Do autism and specific language impairment represent overlapping language disorders? In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 3152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004b). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 7580.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., & Benasich, A. A. (2002). Developmental language learning impairments. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 559579.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., Miller, S., & Jenkins, W. (1998). Language learning impairments: Integrating basic science, technology, and remediation. Experimental Brain Research, 123, 210219.Google Scholar
Thal, D., & Katich, J. (1996). Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment: Does the early bird always catch the worm? In K. N. Cole, P. S. Dale,., & D. J. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (Vol. 6, pp. 128). Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes.
Thal, D., Reilly, J., Seibert, L., Jeffries, R., & Fenson, J. (2004). Language development in children at risk for language impairment: Cross-population comparisons. Brain and Language, 88, 167179.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., & Bates, E. (2003). Connectionism and dynamic systems: Are they really different? Developmental Science, 6, 378391.Google Scholar
Thorpe, K., Rutter, M., & Greenwood, R. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study causes of mild language delay. II. Family interaction risk factors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 342355.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Bates, E. (2001). Language development: The essential readings. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Tomblin, J. B., Hardy, J. C., & Hein, H. A. (1991). Predicting poor communication status in preschool children using risk factors present at birth. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 10961105.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Smith, E., & Zhang, X. (1997). Epidemiology of specific language impairment: Prenatal and perinatal risk factors. Journal of Communication Disorders, 30, 325344.Google Scholar
Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Weiss, A., Catts, H. W., & Weismer, S. E. (2004). Dimensions of individual differences in communication skills among primary grade children. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 5376). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbuam.
Torr, J. (2003). Personality disorder in intellectual disability. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 15, 517521.Google Scholar
Tronick, E. Z., & Beeghly, M. (1999). Prenatal cocaine exposure, child development, and the compromising effects of cumulative risk. Clinics in Perinatology, 26, 151171.Google Scholar
Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Dijkstra, J., & Bus, A. G. (1995). Attachment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Social Development, 4, 115128.Google Scholar
Warfield, M. E., Hauser-Cram, P., Krauss, M. W., & Upshur, C. C. (2000). The effect of early intervention services on maternal well-being. Early Education and Development, 11, 499517.Google Scholar
Weckerly, J., Wulfeck, B., & Reilly, J. (2003). The development of morphosyntactic ability in atypical populations: The acquisition of tag questions in children with early focal lesions and children with specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 88, 190121.Google Scholar
Wulfeck, B., Bates, E., Krupa-Kwiatkowski, M., & Saltzman, D. (2004). Grammaticality sensitivity in children with early focal brain injury and children with specific language impairment. Brain and Language, 88, 215228.Google Scholar
Yirmiya, N., Pilowsky, T., Solomonica-Levy, D., & Shulman, C. (1999). Gaze behavior and theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 333341.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., Hooshyar, N., Klee, T., & Shaffer, M. (1996). Comparisons of the types of child utterances mothers expand with children with language delays and with Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 557567.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1999). Maternal responsivity mediates the relationship between prelinguistic intentional communication and later language. Journal of Early Intervention, 22, 126136.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2004). Early predictors of language in children with and without Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 109, 285300.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. (1971). The retarded child as a whole person. In H. E. Adams & W. K. Boardman (Eds.), Advances in experimental clinical psychology (pp. 47121). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Zukowski, A. (2004). Investigating knowledge of complex syntax: Insights from experimental studies of Williams syndrome. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.