Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:13:47.095Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Symbolic play in congenitally blind children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2005

MARTIN BISHOP
Affiliation:
Tavistock Clinic, London; and University College, London
R. PETER HOBSON
Affiliation:
Tavistock Clinic, London; and University College, London
ANTHONY LEE
Affiliation:
Tavistock Clinic, London; and University College, London

Abstract

There is controversy over the existence and nature of blind children's limitations in symbolic play. In this study we tested 13 5- to 9-year-old congenitally blind children for the ability to symbolize when an adult provided scaffolding for their play. The blind children were selected on the basis that they did not have the syndrome of autism, and they comprised two groups matched for age (MA) and verbal ability on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children who contrasted in their ability to engage in social relations. We also tested a group of sighted children who were MA matched with the more socially able blind children. On the basis of an hypothesis about the social–developmental sources of symbolic play deficits in congenitally blind children, we predicted differences between the socially able and socially impaired groups of blind children in the following respects: the attribution of symbolic meanings to play materials, the ascription of individual roles to play figures, and the anchorage of play in the scenario as presented by the adult. The results accorded with these predictions. Whereas the more socially able blind children showed symbolic play that was very similar to that of sighted children, the MA- and IQ-matched socially impaired blind children were limited in the three aspects of their play. On the other hand, even the socially impaired children showed instances of symbolizing. The findings suggest a way to reconcile conflicting reports of symbolic play deficits in young blind children, and may be relevant for explaining the association between autism and congenital blindness.This study was supported by a PhD studentship from the Mary Kitzinger Trust to Martin Bishop. The Hayward Foundation also contributed financial support. We thank the pupils and staff of the following schools, who were so generous in making the study possible: Dorton House School, Sevenoaks; West of England School, Exeter; Joseph Clarke School, London; Linden Lodge School, Wimbledon; Temple Bank School, Bradford; St. Vincent's School for the Blind, Liverpool; RNIB Sunshine House School, Northwood; and Priestley Smith School, Birmingham.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Andersen, E. S., Dunlea, A., & Kekelis, L. S. (1984). Blind children's language: Resolving some differences. Journal of Child Language 11, 645664.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (Ed.). (1984). Symbolic play: The representation of social understanding. New York: Academic Press.
Brown, R., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Stevenson, J. (1997). Are there “autistic-like” features in congenitally blind children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 693703.Google Scholar
Burlingham, D. (1961). Some notes on the development of the blind. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 16, 121145.Google Scholar
Cass, H. D., Sonksen, P. M., & McConachie, H. R. (1994). Developmental setback in severe visual impairment. Archives of Disability in Childhood 70, 192196.Google Scholar
Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play: An integrative review. Child Development 52, 10951118.Google Scholar
Ferguson, R., & Buultjens, M. (1995). The play behavior of young blind children and its relationship to developmental stages. British Journal of Visual Impairment 13, 100107.Google Scholar
Fraiberg, S. (Ed.). (1977). Insights from the blind. London: Souvenir Press.
Fraiberg, S., & Adelson, E. (1977). Self-representation in language and play. In S. Fraiberg (Ed.), Insights from the blind (pp. 248270). London: Souvenir Press.
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On acquiring knowledge about people and the capacity to pretend: Response to Leslie (1987). Psychological Review 97, 114121.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1993a). Through feeling and sight to self and symbol. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 254279). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hobson, R. P. (1993b). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Erlbaum.
Hobson, R. P. (2000). The grounding of symbols: A social–developmental account. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.), Reasoning and the mind (pp. 1135). Hove: Psychology Press.
Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan.
Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (2003). The pathogenesis of autism: Insights from congenital blindness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 358, 335344.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Brown, R., Minter, M. E., & Lee, A. (1997). “Autism” revisited: The case of congenital blindness. In V. Lewis & G. M. Collis (Eds.), Blindness and psychological development in young children (pp. 99115). Leicester: British Psychological Society.
Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29, 4556.Google Scholar
Hughes, M., Dote–Kwan, J., & Dolendo, J. (1998). A close look at the cognitive play of preschoolers with visual impairments in the home. Exceptional Children 64, 451462.Google Scholar
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159174.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review 94, 412416.Google Scholar
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 6, 325339.Google Scholar
Lewis, V., Norgate, S., Collis, G., & Reynolds, R. (2000). The consequences of visual impairment for children's symbolic and functional play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 18, 449464.Google Scholar
Minter, M. E., Hobson, R. P., & Bishop, M. (1998). Congenital visual impairment and “theory of mind.” British Journal of Developmental Psychology 16, 183196.Google Scholar
Parsons, S. (1986). Function of play in low vision children (Part 2): Emerging patterns of behavior. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 80, 777784.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
Preisler, G. M. (1991). Early patterns of interaction between blind infants and their sighted mothers. Child: Care, Health and Development 17, 6590.Google Scholar
Preisler, G. M. (1993). A descriptive study of blind children in nurseries with sighted children. Child: Care, Health and Development 19, 295315.Google Scholar
Preisler, C. (1995). The development of communication in blind and in deaf infants—Similarities and differences. Child: Care, Health and Development 21, 79110.Google Scholar
Preisler, G., & Palmer, C. (1989). Thoughts from Sweden: The blind child at nursery school with sighted children. Child: Care, Health and Development 15, 4552.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Newhart–Larson, S. (1989). Characteristics of infantile autism in five children with Leber's congenital amaurosis. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 31, 598608.Google Scholar
Rogers, S. J., & Puchalski, C. B. (1984). Development of symbolic play in visually impaired young children. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 3, 5763.Google Scholar
Sandler, A. M., & Hobson, R. P. (2001). On engaging with people in early childhood: The case of congenital blindness. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry 6, 205222.Google Scholar
Sandler, A. M., & Wills, D. M. (1965). Preliminary notes on play and mastery in the blind child. Journal of Child Psychotherapy 1, 719.Google Scholar
Schneekloth, L. H. (1989). Play environments for visually impaired children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 83, 196210.Google Scholar
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988). The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological.
Slade, A., & Wolf, D. P. (1994). Children at play: Clinical and developmental approaches to meaning and representation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, N. R., & Franklin, M. B. (Eds.). (1979). Symbolic functioning in childhood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tait, P. E. (1972). A descriptive analysis of the play of young blind children. Education of the Visually Handicapped 4, 1215.Google Scholar
Troster, H., & Brambring, M. (1994). The play behavior and play materials of blind and sighted infants and preschoolers. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 88, 421432.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1976). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. In J. Bruner, J. Jolly, & K. Sylva (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 537554). Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1984/1963). Symbol formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wills, D. M. (1965). Some observations on blind nursery school children's understanding of their world. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 20, 344364.Google Scholar
Wills, D. M. (1968). Problems of play and mastery in the blind child. British Journal of Medical Psychology 41, 213222.Google Scholar
Wills, D. M. (1979a). “The ordinary devoted mother” and her blind baby. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 34, 3149.Google Scholar
Wills, D. M. (1979b). Early speech development in blind children. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 34, 85117.Google Scholar
Wills, D. M. (1981). Some notes on the application of the diagnostic profile to young blind children. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 36, 217237.Google Scholar
Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 9, 1129.Google Scholar
Winnicott, D. W. (1971). Playing and reality. London: Tavistock.
Wolf, D., & Gardner, H. (1981). On the structure of early symbolisation. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & D. D. Bricker (Eds.), Early language: Acquisition and intervention (pp. 287327). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.
Wolf, D. P., Rygh, J., & Altschuler, J. (1984). Agency and experience: Actions and states in play narratives. In I. Bretherton (Ed.), Symbolic play (pp. 195217). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Wulff, S. B. (1985). The symbolic and object play of children with autism: A review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 15, 139148.Google Scholar