Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T15:45:30.666Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Downward entailment in child Mandarin*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2011

YI (ESTHER) SU*
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Australia
PENG ZHOU
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Australia
STEPHEN CRAIN
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Australia
*
Address for correspondence: Yi (Esther) Su, ARC Centre of Excellence for Cognition and its Disorders, Level 4 Building C5C, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia. e-mail: esther.su@mq.edu.au; sy-esther@hotmail.com

Abstract

There are three hallmarks of core linguistic properties. First, they are expected to be manifested in typologically different languages. Second, they should unify superficially unrelated linguistic phenomena. Third, they are expected to emerge early in the course of language development, all things being equal (Crain, 1991). The present study investigates a candidate for a core linguistic property, namely the semantic property of downward entailment. We report the findings of two experimental studies of children's knowledge of downward entailment. These experiments explore two different aspects of downward entailment, in a study with Mandarin-speaking children. Taken together with previous research findings, the results of the present study support the conclusion that downward entailment is a core property of human languages.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by the Australian Research Council Grant DP0879842 to Stephen Crain and Rosalind Thornton. It was also partially supported by the Chinese National Science Grant 30770767 to Linyan Su and the Chinese National Social Science Grant 09BYY022 to Liqun Gao. We thank Nobu Akagi, Anna Notley, Francesco Ursini and especially Thomas Lee and Rosalind Thornton for valuable suggestions; we thank the anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments. We would also like to thank those children and adults who participated in the experiments for their support and cooperation in the research.

References

REFERENCES

Boster, C. T. & Crain, S. (1993). On children's understanding of every and or. In Smith, C. (ed.), Proceedings of Early Cognition and the Transition to Language, 123. Austin: University of Texas.Google Scholar
Cairns, H. S., Schlisselberg, G., Waltzman, D. & McDaniel, D. (2006). Development of a metalinguistic skill: Judging the grammaticality of sentences. Communication Disorders Quarterly 27, 213–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S. (1991). On the typology of wh-questions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Cheng, L. L.-S. (1994). Wh-words as polarity items. Chinese Languages and Linguistics 2, 615–40.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Foris: Dordrecht.Google Scholar
Conroy, A. & Thornton, R. (2005). Children's knowledge of Principle C in discourse. In Otsu, Y. (ed.), Proceedings of the 6th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, 6994. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, 597650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. (2008). The interpretation of disjunction in Universal Grammar. Language and Speech 51, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crain, S. (2009). Sentence scope. In Bavin, E. L. (ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language, 301320. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Gardner, A., Gualmini, A. & Rabbin, B. (2002). Children's command of negation. In Otsu, Y. (ed.), Proceedings of the 3rd Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, 7195. Tokyo: Hituzi Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Crain, S., Goro, T. & Thornton, R. (2006). Language acquisition is language change. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 35, 3149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crain, S., Gualmini, A. & Pietroski, P. (2005). Brass tacks in linguistic theory: Innate grammatical principles. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S. & Stich, S. (eds.), The innate mind: Structure and content, 175–97. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & Khlentzos, D. (2008). Is logic innate? Biolinguistics 2, 2456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & Khlentzos, D. (2010). The logic instinct. Mind and Language 25, 3065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & McKee, C. (1985). The acquisition of structural restrictions on anaphora. In Berman, S., Choe, J. W. & McDonough, J. (eds.), Proceedings of NELS 15, 94110. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Pietroski, P. (2001). Nature, nurture and Universal Grammar. Linguistics and Philosophy 24, 139–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A guide to experiments on the acquisition of syntax and semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S. & Thornton, R. (2006). Acquisition of syntax and semantics. In Traxler, M. & Gersbacher, M. (eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics, 3rd edn.1073–110. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crain, S., Thornton, R. & Khlentzos, D. (2009). The case of the missing generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics 20, 145–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, H. T. & Kirkpatrick, A. G. (1999). Metalinguistic awareness in children: A developmental progression. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 28, 313–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giannakidou, A. (2011). Negative polarity items and positive polarity items: Licensing, variation, and compositionality. In Heusinger, K. v., Maienborn, C. & Portner, P. (eds), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning, 2nd edn, 1660–772. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (2003). Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Science 7, 219–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, A. (2006). Constructions at work. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goro, T. & Akiba, S. (2004). The acquisition of disjunction and positive polarity in Japanese. In Chand, V., Kelleher, A., Rodriguez, A. & Schmeiser, B. (eds), Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 251–64. Summerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (eds), Syntax and semantics, Vol. 3: Speech Acts, 4158. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gualmini, A. & Crain, S. (2002). Why no child or adult must learn de Morgan's laws. In Skarabela, B., Fish, S. & Do, A. H.-J. (eds), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 243–54. Somerville MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gualmini, A. & Crain, S. (2005). The structure of children's linguistic knowledge. Linguistic Inquiry 36, 463–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gualmini, A., Meroni, L. & Crain, S. (2003). An asymmetric universal in child language. In Weisgerber, M. (ed.), Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung VI, 136–48. Konstanz: Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft der Universitat Konstanz.Google Scholar
Higginbotham, J. (1991). Either/or. In Sherer, T. (ed.), Proceedings of NELS 21, 143–55. Amherst, MA: GLSA Publications.Google Scholar
Horn, L. (1972). On the semantic properties of logical operators in English. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Horn, L. (1989). A natural history of negation. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Huang, C.-T. J. (1982). Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Jing, C., Crain, S. & Hsu, C.-F. (2005). The interpretation of focus in Chinese: Child vs. adult language. In Otsu, Y. (ed.), Proceedings of the 6th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, 165–90. Tokyo: Hituzi Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Kadmon, N. & Landman, F. (1993). Any. Linguistics and Philosophy 16, 353422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kazanina, N., & Phillips, C. (2001). Coreference in child Russian: Distinguishing syntactic and discourse constraints. In Anna, H.-J.Do, Laura Domínguez & Johansen, Aimee (eds), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 413–24. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Kiguchi, H. & Thornton, R. (2004). Binding principles and ACD constructions in child grammars. Syntax 7, 234–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E. S. (1964). Negation in English. In Fodor, Jerry A. and Katz, Jerrold J. (eds), The structure of language, 246323. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Koster, C. & van der Wal, S. (1995). Acquiring a negative polarity verb. In Verrips, M. & Wijnen, F. (eds), Papers from the German–Dutch Colloquium on Language Acquisition, 109126. Amsterdam: Universiteit van Amsterdam, Instituut voor Algemene Taalwetenschap.Google Scholar
Krifka, M. (1995). The semantics and pragmatics of polarity items. Linguistic Analysis 25, 209257.Google Scholar
Ladusaw, W. (1979). Negative polarity as inherent scope relations. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas.Google Scholar
Li, Y.-H. A. (1992). Indefinite wh in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 1, 125–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, J.-W. (1996). Polarity licensing and wh-phrase quantification in Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar
Lin, J.-W. (1998). On existential polarity wh-phrases in Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 7, 219–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Morris, B. J. (2008). Logically speaking: Evidence for item-based acquisition of the connectives AND & OR. Journal of Cognition and Development 9, 6788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Notley, A., Zhou, P., Jensen, B. & Crain, S. (in press). Children's interpretation of disjunction in the scope of ‘before’: A comparison of English and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language.Google Scholar
O'Leary, C. & Crain, S. (1994). Negative polarity items (a positive result) positive polarity items (a negative result). Paper presented at the 19th Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Paris, S. G. (1973). Comprehension of language connectives and propositional logical relationships. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 16, 278–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Partee, B. H., Meulen, A. G. ter & Wall, R. E. (1990). Mathematical methods in linguistics. Dordrecht/Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Pietroski, P. M. & Crain, S. (in press). The language faculty. In Margolis, E., Samuels, R. & Stich, S. P. (eds), The handbook of philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. & Scholz, B. (2002). Empirical assessment of the stimulus poverty argument. The Linguistic Review 19, 950.Google Scholar
Saywitz, K. & Cherry-Wilkinson, L. (1982). Age-related differences in metalinguistic awareness. In Kuczaj, S. (ed.), Language development, 229–48. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Su, Y. & Crain, S. (2009). Disjunction and universal quantification in child Mandarin. In Otsu, Y. (ed.), Proceedings of the 10th Tokyo Conference on Psycholinguistics, 265–89. Tokyo: Hituzi Syobo Publishing.Google Scholar
Szabolcsi, A. (2002). Hungarian disjunctions and positive polarity. In Kenesei, I. & Siptár, P. (eds), Approaches to Hungarian: Levels and structures, 217–41. Budapest: Akademiai Kiado.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
van der Wal, S. (1996). Negative polarity items and negation: Tandem acquisition. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Gröningen.Google Scholar
van der Wouden, T. (1994). Negative contexts. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Gröningen.Google Scholar
Zwarts, F. (1995). Nonveridical contexts. Linguistic Analysis 25, 286312.Google Scholar
Zwarts, F. (1998). Three types of polarity. In Hamm, F. & Hinrichs, E. (eds), Plurality and quantification, 177238. Dordrecht/Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, P. & Crain, S. (2011). Children's knowledge of the quantifier dou in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 40(3), 155–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed