Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T08:23:32.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Turn taking affects the quality of infant vocalizations*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Kathleen Bloom*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
Ann Russell*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
Karen Wassenberg*
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University
*
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, CanadaB3H 4J1.
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, CanadaB3H 4J1.
Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, CanadaB3H 4J1.

Abstract

Two groups of 20 infants aged 0; 3 experienced either conversational turn taking or random responsiveness of an adult. All infant vocalizations were counted and then each was categorized as a speech-like (syllabic) sound or a nonspeech-like (vocalic) sound. The results of this experiment indicated that turn taking caused changes in the quality of infant vocal sounds. When the adult maintained a give-and-take pattern, the infant produced a higher ratio of syllabic/vocalic sounds. The effect of turn taking on infant vocalizations was discussed in terms of its possible adaptive value for adult responsiveness.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1987

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

The authors express their appreciation to Lois Bloom, James Enns, Philip Dunham, Hildy Ross, and Denise Sommerfeld for their comments and advice concerning the manuscript, Susan Davis and Elsa Lo for their technical assistance, and Heather Walker for serving as one of the experimenters. Special thanks are extended to the parents and infants who so generously participated in this study and to the staff of the Grace Maternity Hospital who helped with the recruitment of participants. This research was made possible by grants (410–83–0660 and 410–85–0227) to the first author from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada.

References

Bateson, M. C. (1975) Mother-infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences 263. 101–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, J. & Cunningham, C. C. (1983) Development of early vocal behaviours and interactions in Down's Syndrome and nonhandicapped infant-mother pairs. Developmental Psychology 19. 322–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1974) Eye contact as a setting event for infant learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 17. 250–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K. (1975) Social elicitation of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 20. 51–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1977 a) Operant baseline procedures suppress infant social behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 23. 128–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1977 b) Patterning of infant vocal behaviour. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 23. 367–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. (1979) Evaluation of infant vocal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27. 6070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, K. (1984) Distinguishing between social reinforcement and social elicitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 38. 93102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, K. & Esposito, A. (1975) Social conditioning and its proper control procedures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19. 209–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L. (1983) Discussion: of continuity and discontinuity, the magic of language development. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication: issues and implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976) Adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology 8. 521–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975) From communication to language–a psychological perspective. Cognition 3. 255–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delack, J. B. (1976) Aspects of infant speech development in the first year of life. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 21. 1737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Odorico, L. (1984) Non-segmental features in prelinguistic communications: an analysis of some types of infant cry and non-cry vocalizations. Journal of Child Language 11. 1727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elias, G., Broerse, J., Hayes, A. & Jackson, K. (1984) Comments on the use of conversational features in the study of the vocalization behaviours of mothers and infants. International Journal of Behavioural Development 7. 177–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1964) Baby talk in six languages. American Anthropologist 66. 103–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernald, A. (1984) The perceptual and affective salience of mother's speech to infants. In Feagans, L., Garvey, C., Golinkoff, R., Greenberg, M. T., Harding, C. & Bohannon, J. N. (eds.) The origins and growth of communication. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (1977) Temporal organization in mother-infant face-to-face interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interactions. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A. (1981) The ontogeny of gestural communication: the first six months. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Fraiberg, S. (1977) Insights from the blind: comparative studies of blind and sighted infants. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Freedle, R. & Lewis, M. (1977) Prelinguistic conversation. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (eds), Interaction, conversation and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J. & Feldstein, S. (1970) Rhythms of dialogue. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1971) Change and continuity in infancy. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977) Toward the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1979) Thickening thin data: the maternal role in developing communication and language. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1982) The mental and social life of babies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. & Fogel, A. (1980) The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology 16. 454–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, R. D. (1981) Articulatory-acoustic perspectives on speech development. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Kent, R. D. & Murray, A. D. (1982) Acoustic features of infant vocalic utterances at 3, 6, and 9 months. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 72. 353–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. & Lee-Painter, S. (1974) An interactional approach to the mother-infant dyad. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (eds), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Locke, J. L. (1983). Phonological Acquisition and Change. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Martin, J. A., Maccoby, E. E., Baran, K. W. & Jacklin, C. N. (1981) Sequential analysis of mother-child interaction at 18 months: a comparison of microanalytic methods. Developmental Psychology 17. 146–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maskarinec, A. S., Cairns, G. F., Butterfield, E. C. & Weamer, D. K. (1981) Longitudinal observations of individual infant's vocalizations. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 46. 267–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973) Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38. No. 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Netsell, R. (1981) The acquisition of speech motor control: a perspective with directions for research. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1980) The emergence of the sounds of speech in early infancy. In Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Kavanagh, J. F. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Child phonology. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1981) Infant vocalizations: exploration and reflexivity. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J. & Ross, C. (1976) Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language 3. 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1981) Musical elements in the infant's vocalization: their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. In Lipsitt, L. P. (ed.), Advances in infancy research. Vol. 1. New Jersey: Ablex.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951) Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. (1978) Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language 5. 391401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, M. P. M. (1971) A comment on the social context of mother-infant interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), The origins of human social relations. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Robson, K. S. (1967) The role of eye-to-eye contact in maternal-infant attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 8. 1325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roe, K. V. (1975) Amount of infant vocalization as a function of age: some cognitive implications. Child Development 46. 936–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, H. S. & Lollis, S. P. (in press) Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Sackett, G. P. (1979) The lag sequential analysis of contingency and cyclicity in behavioural interaction research. In Osofsky, J. D. (ed.), Handbook of infant development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50. 696735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sameroff, A. J., & Cavanagh, P. J. (1979) Learning in infancy: a developmental perspective. In Osofsky, J. D. (ed.), Handbook of infant development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. & Ochs, E. (1983) A cultural perspective on the transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication. In Golinkoff, R. M. (ed.), The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication: issues and implications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sheppard, W. C. & Lane, H. L. (1968) Development of prosodic features of infant vocalizing. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 11. 94108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1977) The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4. 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. E. (1978) Features of infant sounds: the emergence of cooing. Journal of Child Language 5. 379–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, R. E. (1981 a) Infant vocalization: a comprehensive view. Infant Mental Health Journal 2. 118–28.3.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stark, R. E. (1981 b) Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. New York: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Stark, R. E., Rose, S. N. & McLagen, M. (1975) Features of infant sounds: the first eight weeks of life. Journal of Child Language 2. 205–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N., (1974) Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviours. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (eds), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B. & Bennet, S. L. (1975) Vocalizing in unison and in alternation: two kinds of communication within the mother-infant dyad. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences 263. 89100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., Barnett, R. K., & MacKain, K. (1983) The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes. Journal of Child Language 10. 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stockman, I. J., Woods, D. R. & Tishman, A. (1981) Listener agreement on phonetic segments in early infant vocalizations. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 10. 593617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomkins, S. (1962) Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. I. The positive affect. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Vietze, P. M., Abernathy, S. R., Ashe, M. L. & Faulstich, G. (1978) Contingent interaction between mothers and their developmentally delayed infants. In Sackett, G. P. (ed.), Observing behaviour. Vol. I. Theory and applications in mental retardation. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (1978) The development of communication. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Watson, J. (1972) Smiling, cooing and ‘the game’. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 18. 323–39.Google Scholar
Wolff, P. H. (1969) The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), The determinants of infant behaviour. Vol. 4. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Wolff, P. H. (1981) Discussion of social development and communicative behaviour in infancy. In Stark, R. E. (ed.), Language behaviour in infancy and early childhood. Amsterdam: Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P. R., Kagan, J. & Hartmann, R. (1975) Excitement and boredom as determinants of vocalization in infants. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 126. 107–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar