Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T06:57:24.975Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does imitation facilitate the acquisition of grammar? Evidence from a study of autistic, Down's syndrome and normal children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Helen Tager-Flusberg*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts
Susan Calkins
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
*
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA 02125, USA.

Abstract

This paper re-opens the question of whether imitation plays a significant role in the acquisition of grammar. Data for this study came from four samples of naturalistic mother-child speech taken over the course of one year from four autistic, four Down's syndrome and four normal children, covering a range of MLU stages. In general, autistic children used more formulaic language, including imitations, than Down's syndrome children, who in turn used more than the normal children. Comparisons of imitative and spontaneous corpora from the same transcripts were made using MLU and the Index of Productive Syntax. The main findings were that, with few exceptions, spontaneous speech utterances were longer, and contained more advanced grammatical constructions than did the imitation utterances. These findings held across all three groups of subjects. We conclude that imitation does not facilitate grammatical development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was generously supported by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (RO1 HD 18833). Portions of this study were presented at the 1986 Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association. We thank Gail Andrick, Ann Chadwick-Dias, and Karen Price for their help in preparing the transcripts. We also wish to extend our sincere appreciation to the children and their families who participated in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L. (1988). From first words to grammar: individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York. C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (eds) (1964). The acquisition of language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 29. No. 92.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Hood, L. & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: if, when, and why. Cognitive Psychology 6. 380420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Lightbown, P. & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 40. No. 160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology 8. 521–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohannon, J. N. & Warren-Leubecker, A. (1989). Theoretical approaches to language acquisition. In Gleason, J. B. (ed.), The development of language. Second edition. Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S., Snyder, L. & Bates, E. (1983). Individual differences at 20 months: analytic and holistic strategies in language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10. 293320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casby, M. W. (1986). A pragmatic perspective of repetition in child language. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 15. 127–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. (1977). What's the use of imitation? Journal of Child Language 4. 341–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, R. (1978). Some even simpler ways to learn to talk. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Coggins, T. E. & Morrison, J. A. (1981). Spontaneous imitations of Down's syndrome children: a lexical analysis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 24. 303–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dooley, J. (1976). Language acquisition and Down's syndrome: a study of early semantics and syntax. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Ervin, S. (1964). Imitation and structural change in children's language. In Lenneberg, E. (ed.), New directions in the study of language. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J., Bever, T. & Garrett, M. (1974). The psychology of language: an introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. (1984). Language acquisition of Down's syndrome children: production and comprehension. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Hardy-Brown, K. (1983). Universals and individual differences: disentangling two approaches to the study of language acquisition. Developmental Psychology 19. 610–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howlin, P. (1982). Echolalic and spontaneous phrase speech in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 23. 281–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jespersen, O. (1922). Language: its nature, development, and origin. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Kemp, J. & Dale, P. (1973). Spontaneous imitation and free speech: a grammatical comparison. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
McWhinney, B. & Snow, C. (1985). The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language 12. 271–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. & Chapman, R. (1985). Systematic analysis of language transcripts: user's guide. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin, Language Analysis Laboratory.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. L. (1977). Processes and products of imitation: additional evidence that imitation is progressive. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 6. 187202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moerk, E. L. (1980). Relationships between parental input frequencies and children's language acquisition: A reanalysis of Brown's data. Journal of Child Language 7. 105–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38. No. 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology 17. 170–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. M. ( 1977). Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Language 53. 560–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1983). The units of language acquisition. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1987). The role of imitation in the developing syntax of a blind child. Text 7. 289311.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1981). On the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 8. 477–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prizant, B. (1983). Language acquisition and communication behavior in autism: toward an understanding of the ‘whole’ of it. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 48. 296307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reger, Z. (1986). The functions of imitation in child language. Applied Psycholinguistics 7. 323–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. (1980). Verbal imitation by Down's syndrome and nonretarded children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 85. 318–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. (1985). Measuring syntactic development: the Index of Productive Syntax. Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto, Canada.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. (1990). Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics 11, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, N. (1975). Echolalic speech in childhood autism. Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 1439–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. Child Development 43. 549–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language 8. 205–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. & Bates, E. (1984). Individual differences: a cross-language approach. Workshop presented at the Stanford Child Language Forum, Stanford, California.Google Scholar
Stine, E. & Bohannon, J. N. (1983). Interactions, imitations, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10. 589603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1982). Pragmatic development and its implications for social interaction in autistic children. In Park, D. (ed.), The proceedings of the 1981 international conference on autism. Washington DC: NSAC.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., Baumberger, T., Anderson, M. & Chadwick-Dias, A. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed