Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-07T14:36:11.085Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interactive communication between children: learning how to make language work in dialogue*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Anne H. Anderson*
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Aileen Clark
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
James Mullin
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
*
H.C.R.C., Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, G12 9YR, UK.

Abstract

In this paper we investigate the development of interactive communication skills in 170 children aged seven to thirteen. Using a communication task that allows extended dialogues between pairs of young speakers, we are able to assess both the overall communicative success achieved by any pair; and from an analysis of the dialogues we can identify several interactive strategies that characterize older and more successful communicators. Successful communication involves the active involvement of both participants: asking and answering questions, volunteering information and responding sensitively to contributions from their partners. In contrast to the process of language acquisition, the development of these interactive skills takes place over an extended time period and subjects vary greatly in their ability to communicate effectively. For many analyses differences in the communicative success achieved by subjects differ more within than between age groups. For example, a substantial minority of our oldest subjects communicate no better than children six years younger

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The research reported in this paper was supported by a grant from the British Economic and Social Research Council (RC 00232458) and the researchers gratefully acknowledge their assistance. We thank the staff and pupils of the participating schools for their help in this study.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, A. H. & Lynch, T. (1988). Listening. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Andersen, A. H., Clark, A. & Mullin, J. (1991). Introducing information in dialogues: forms of introduction chosen by young speakers and the responses elicited from young listeners. Journal of Child Language 18, 663–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, A. H., Garrod, A., Clark, A., Boyle, E. & Mullin, J. (1992). The Human Communication Research Centre dialogue database. Journal of Child Language 19, 711–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, E., Camioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. In Ochs, E. & Schiefflin, B. (eds), Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Brown, G., Anderson, A. H., Shillcock, R. & Yule, G. (1984). Teaching talk. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Brown, G. & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1985). Language use and language users. In Lindsey, G. & Aronson, E. (eds), The handbook of social psychology. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Joshi, A. K., Webber, B. L. & Sag, I. A. (eds), Elements of discourse understanding. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Schaeffer, E. (1987). Collaborating on contributions to conversations. Language and Cognitive Processes 2, 1941.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition 22, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condon, W. S. & Sander, L. W. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition. Science 183, 99101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demetras, M. J., Post, K. N. & Snow, C. E. (1986). Feedback to first language learners: the role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language 13, 275–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, W. & Pechmann, T. (1987). Social interaction and the development of definite descriptions. Cognition 27, 181218.Google Scholar
Dickson, W. P. (1981). Referential communication activities in research and the curriculum: a meta analysis. In Dickson, W. P. (ed.), Children's oral communication skills. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1970). Discourse agreement: how children answer questions. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Speer, J. R., Green, F. L. & August, D. L. (1981). The development of comprehension monitoring and knowledge about communication. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 46, 5, No. 192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, T. (1981). Contingent query sequences within adult-child discourse. Journal of Child Language 8, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S. & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: a study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition 27, 181218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golinkoff, R. M. (1986). ‘I beg your pardon?’: the preverbal negotiation of failed messages. Journal of Child Language 13, 455–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organisation: interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. A. (1978). Definiteness and indefiniteness – a study in reference and grammatical prediction. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Houghton, G. & Isard, S. (1987). Why to speak, what to say and how to say it. In Morris, P. (ed.), Modelling cognition. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ironsmith, M. & Whitehurst, G. (1978). How children learn to listen: the effects of modelling feedback styles on children's performance in referential communication. Developmental Psychology 14, 546–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kartunnen, L. (1968). What makes noun phrases definite? Santa Monica: The Rand Corporation.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. M. & Glucksberg, S. (1969). The development of communication competence as a function of age. Child Development 40, 255–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcos, H. & Bernicot, J. (in press). Addressee co-operation and request reformulation in young children. Journal of Child Language 21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McTear, M. F. (1987). Communication failure: a developmental perspective. In Reilly, R. G. (ed.), Communication failure in dialogue and discourse. Amsterdam: North-Holland-Elsevier.Google Scholar
Olsen-Fulero, L. (1982). Style and stability in mother conversational behaviour: a study of individual differences. Journal of Child Language 9, 543–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, G. L., Wellman, H. M. & Karabenick, J. D. (1985). Comprehension monitoring in pre-school children. Child Development 56, 654–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, E. (1981). Conversational tactics and the advancement of the child's understanding about referential communication. In Robinson, W. (ed.), Communication in development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking in conversation. Language 50, 696735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schober, M. J. & Clark, H. H. (1989). Understanding by addressees and overhearers. Cognitive Psychology 21, 211–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shatz, M. (1983). Communication. In Mussen, P. (ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. III. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. & O'Reilly, A. W. (1990). Conversational or communicative skill? A reassessment of two-year olds' behaviour in miscommunication episodes. Journal of Child Language 17, 131–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, J. & Coulthart, M. (1975). Towards an analysis of discourse. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steffenson, M. (1978). Satisfying inquisitive adults: some simple methods of answering yes/no questions. Journal of Child Language 5, 221–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and co-operation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Webber, B. L. (1979). A formal approach to discourse anaphora. New York: Garland.Google Scholar