Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T12:03:11.939Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Turn the page please: situation-specific language acquisition*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Catherine E. Snow
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education
Beverly A. Goldfield
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education

Abstract

The present paper investigates a strategy for language acquisition adopted by one child, and the usefulness of book-reading in supporting that strategy. Conversations between this child and his mother around picture-book reading were recorded and recurrent discussions of the same picture were analysed. A coding scheme was devised which identified each speaker's contribution to the exchange of information about a particular picture. By examining recurrent picture discussions, it was possible to trace the child's acquisition of the linguistic means for talking about a given picture. It was found that specific lexical items and constructions used to talk about a picture frequently recurred in subsequent discussions, and that the child learned many of these same items and constructions. Furthermore, he was most likely to acquire what he had heard his mother say about a picture if he had repeated it in an earlier discussion. The usefulness of routines for such situation-specific learning and the implications of the situation-specific approach for future investigations of input language are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, A. B., Teale, W. H. & Estrada, E. (1980). Low income children's preschool literacy experiences: some naturalistic observations. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition 2. 5965.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Morehead, D. &Morehead, A. (eds), Directions in normal and deficient child language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Braine, M. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 41, 1.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975 a). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang 2. 121.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975 b). From communication to language – a psychological perspective. Cognition 3. 255–87.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. & Sherwood, V. (1976). Peekaboo and the learning of rule structures. In Bruner, J. S.,Jolly, A. &Sylva, K. (eds). Play: its role in development and evolution. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1974). Performing without competence. JChLang 1. 110.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1976). What's the use of imitation?. JChLang 4. 341–58.Google Scholar
Cross, T. (1978). Motherese: its association with the rate of syntactic acquisition in young children. In Waterson, N. &Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ferrier, L. (1978). Some observations of error in context. In Waterson, N. &Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Filmore, L. W. (1979). Individual differences in second language acquisition. In Fillmore, C. J.,Kempler, D. & Wang, W. S.-Y. (eds), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mother's speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. JChLang 6. 423–42.Google Scholar
Haselkorn, S. (1981). The development of the requests of young children from nonverbal strategies to the power of language. Unpublished Ed.D. thesis, Harvard Graduate School of Education.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. & Chalkley, M. A. (1980). The internal language of children's syntax: the ontogenesis and representation of syntactic categories. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language. Vol. 2. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. &Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1980 a). Picture book reading in mother–infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. ChDev 51. 587–90.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. (1980 b). The ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching. JChLang 7. 565–73.Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. JChLang 5. 115.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. (1977). Language learning strategies: does the whole equal the sum of the parts? Lg 53 560–73.Google Scholar
Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. S. (1978). Games, social exchange, and the acquisition of language. JChLang 5. 391402.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1981). The uses of imitation. JChLang 8. 205–12.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (in press). Saying it again: the role of expanded and deferred imitations in language acquisition. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 4. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Dubber, C. & DeBlauw, A. (1981). Routines in parent–child interaction. In Feagans, L. &Farran, D. (eds), The language of children reared in poverty: implications for evaluation and intervention. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wheeler, M. P. (1983). Context-related age changes in mothers' speech: joint book reading. JChLang 10. 259–63.Google Scholar