Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T15:46:31.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child responsiveness to mother questions of varying type and presentation*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lynda Olsen-Fulero
Affiliation:
Antioch College
Jill Conforti
Affiliation:
Antioch College

Abstract

Although questions vary considerably with respect to the responses appropriate to them and the motivations intrinsic to them, little attention has yet been given to the potential effects of these factors on the power of questions to perform turn-allocating and response-eliciting functions. The present study attempts to clarify how such variables as motivation, constraint, difficulty and function may affect the child's responsiveness to mother questions. Results suggest that functionally defined question types are differentiable not only by their relative power to elicit a response from the child, but also by the manner in which they are presented. Implications of these results in the context of mother–child conversation and its effects are considered.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1983

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bell, R. (1971). Stimulus control of parent or caretaker behavior by off-spring. DevPsych 4. 6372.Google Scholar
Berninger, G. & Garvey, C. (1981). Complementary balance in the use of the interrogative form by nursery school dyads. JChLang 8. 297311.Google ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Blount, B. G. (1977). Ethnography and caretaker–child interaction. In Snow, C. E. & Ferguson, C. A. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Brazelton, B., Koslowski, B. & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity. In Lewis, M. &Rosenblum, L. (eds), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Carew, J., Chan, I. & Halfar, C. (1975). Observed intellectual competence and tested intelligence: their roots in the young child's transactions with his environment. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,Denver,Colorado.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. (1977). The clarification request as a feature of adult interactive styles with young children. LangSoc 6. 183207.Google Scholar
Cross, T. (1978). Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In Waterson, N. &Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Duncan, S. & Fiske, D. (1977). Face-to-face interaction: research, methods, and theory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1970). Discourse agreement: how children answer questions. In Hayes, J. (ed), Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate! In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. & Miller, W. (1977). Early discourse: some questions about questions. In Lewis, M. &Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. JChLang 6. 423–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Garnica, O. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In Snow, C. &Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. JChLang 2. 4160.Google Scholar
Garvey, C. (1977). Contingent queries. In Lewis, M. &Rosenblum, L. (eds), Interaction, conversation, and the development of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Howe, C. (1981). Acquiring language in a conversational Context. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J. & Feldstein, S. (1970). Rhythms of dialogue. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kobashiga, B. (1968). Repetitions in a mother's speech to her child. Language, society and the child. Working Paper No. 13, Language Behavior Research Laboratory, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Lennard, H. & Bernstein, A. (1969). Patterns in human interaction. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Matarazzo, J., Wiens, A., Matarazzo, R. & Saslow, G. (1968). In Slien, J. (ed.), Research in psychotherapy, 3. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
McDonald, L. & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behaviour ass function of interactional intent. JChLang 9. 337–58.Google Scholar
Mishler, E. (1975 a). Studies in dialogue and discourse: an exponential law of successive questioning. LangSoc 4. 3151.Google Scholar
Mishler, E. (1975 b). Studies in dialogue and discourse. II. Type of discourse initiated by and sustained through questioning. JPsycholingRes 4. 99121.Google Scholar
Mishler, E. (1978). Studies in dialogue and discourse. III. Utterance structure and utterance function in interrogative sequences. JPsycholingRes 7. 279305.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. (1976). Processes of language teaching and training in the interaction of mother–child dyads. ChDev 47. 1064–78.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1977). Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N.,Pisoni, D. & Potts, G. (eds), Cognitive theory, 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Olsen-Fulero, L. (1982). Style and stability in mother conversational behaviour: a study of individual differences. JChLang 9. 543–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Pawlby, S. (1977). Imitative interaction. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. & Jefferson, G. (1978). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. In Schenkein, J. (ed.), Studies in the organization of conversational interaction. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Savić, S. (1975). Aspects of adult–child communication: the problem of question acquisition. JChLang 2. 255–60.Google Scholar
Shatz, M. (1978). Children's comprehension of their mothers' question-directives. JChLang 5. 3946.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (1956). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. JChLang 4. 122.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1974). Mother and infant at play: the dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors. In Lewis, M. &Rosenblum, L. (eds), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Tamir, L. (1980). Interrogatives in dialogue: case study of mother and child 16–19 months. JPsycholingRes 9. 407–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Winer, B. (1971). Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar