Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-qsmjn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T07:30:37.076Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Content and context in early lexical development*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lynn S. Snyder
Affiliation:
University of Denver
Elizabeth Bates
Affiliation:
University of Colorado
Inge Bretherton
Affiliation:
University of Colorado

Abstract

Investigators have targeted the onset of reference in children at varying points on the developmental continuum. Their viewpoints seem to be related to issues of the contextual flexibility, content and composition of the lexicon in comprehension and production. This study considered all of the variables in the early vocabularies of 32 children with a mean age of 1; 1·7. An intensive maternal interview was used to obtain data on the words comprehended and produced by the children and the contexts which supported their interpretation and use. The findings pointed to a relative independence between the domains of comprehension and production. Vocabulary size was related to all contextually flexible items in production while only related to the contextually flexible predicates in comprehension. Further, there was evidence for an early version of the referential style that has been reported at later stages of development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. MPQ 21. 205–26.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Snyder, L., Bretherton, I. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols in language and action: similarities and differences. PRCLD 17. 106–18.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1979). Early lexical development: comprehension and production. JChLang 6. 183200.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1976). Semantic factors in the acquisition of rules for word use and sentence construction. In Morehead, D. & Morehead, A. (eds), Normal and deficient child language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. JChLang 2. 119.Google Scholar
Carter, A. (1975). The transformation of sensorimotor morphemes into words: a case study of the development of ‘more’ and ‘mine’. JChLang 2. 233–50.Google Scholar
DeLaguna, G. (1927). Speech: its function and development. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1975). Holophrase, speech acts, and language universals. JChLang 2. 2140.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1978). Cognition and communication in language acquisition and development. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Seligtnan, M. & Gelman, R. (1976). Language in the two-year-old. Cognition 4. 189202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harding, C. & Golinkoff, R. (1977). The origins of intentional vocalizations in pre-linguistic infants. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Horgan, D. (1977). Individual differences in the rate of language acquisition. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). Origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: the Loyola Symposium. Potomac, Md.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1978). Sensorimotor intelligence and language development. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leopold, W. F. (1949). Speech development of a bilingual child: a linguist's record. Evanston, Ill.: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38, No. 149.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1978). How children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language. In Sigler, R. S. (ed.), Children's thinking: what develops? Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1979). Explorations in the development of a functional semantic system. In Collins, W. (ed.), Child psychology, Vol. 12. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Oviatt, S. (1979). The developing awareness of linguistic generality in 9–17 month old infants. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development,New Orleans.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1980). Overextension in early language development. JChLang 8. 321–35.Google Scholar
Sugarman-Bell, S. (1978). A description of the communicative development in the prelanguage child. In Markova, I. (ed.), The social context of language. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I. & Camaioni, L. (1979). First words in language and action: a qualitative look. In Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V., The emergence of symbols. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). The blue and the brown books. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar