Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-xxrs7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T17:28:42.874Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 September 2017

LARA J. PIERCE*
Affiliation:
Boston Children's Hospital and Harvard Medical School
FRED GENESEE
Affiliation:
McGill University
AUDREY DELCENSERIE
Affiliation:
Université de Montréal
GARY MORGAN
Affiliation:
City University of London
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Lara J. Pierce, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, 1 Autumn Street, Boston, MA 02115. E-mail: lara.pierce@childrens.harvard.edu or lara.pierce@mcgill.ca

Abstract

In order to build complex language from perceptual input, children must have access to a powerful information processing system that can analyze, store, and use regularities in the signal to which the child is exposed. In this article, we propose that one of the most important parts of this underlying machinery is the linked set of cognitive and language processing components that comprise the child's developing working memory (WM). To examine this hypothesis, we explore how variations in the timing, quality, and quantity of language input during the earliest stages of development are related to variations in WM, especially phonological WM (PWM), and in turn language learning outcomes. In order to tease apart the relationships between early language experience, WM, and language development, we review research findings from studies of groups of language learners who clearly differ with respect to these aspects of input. Specifically, we consider the development of PWM in children with delayed exposure to language, that is, children born profoundly deaf and exposed to oral language following cochlear implantation and internationally adopted children who have delayed exposed to the adoption language; children who experience impoverished language input, that is, children who experience early bouts of otitis media and signing deaf children born to nonsigning hearing parents; and children with enriched early language input, that is, simultaneous bilinguals and second language learners.

Type
Keynote Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2009). Age of onset and nativelikeness in a second language: Listener perception versus linguistic scrutiny. Language Learning, 59, 249306. doi:10.1111/j. 1467-9922.2009.00507.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 403414.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Adams, A. M., & Willis, C. (2005). Working memory abilities in children with special educational needs. Educational and Child Psychology, 22, 5667.Google Scholar
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 16981716. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x Google Scholar
Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41, 675693. doi:10.1080/13682820500442602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417423. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 189208.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. A. (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (pp. 4790). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baker, S. A., Idsardi, W. J., Golinkoff, R. M., & Petitto, L. A. (2005). The perception of hand shapes in American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition, 33, 887904.Google Scholar
Barone, P., Lecassagne, L., & Kral, A. (2013). Reorganization of the connectivity of cortical field DZ in congenitally deaf cat. PLOS ONE, 8, e60093 Google Scholar
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857894. doi:10.1017/S0142716414000174 Google Scholar
Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. F. (2007). Early word-object associations and later language development. First Language, 27, 315328.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (1999). Second language acquisition and the critical period hypothesis. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128, 105119.Google Scholar
Bradlow, A. R., Pisoni, D. B., Akahane-Yamada, R., & Tohkura, Y. I. (1997). Training Japanese listeners to identify English/r/and/l: IV. Some effects of perceptual learning on speech production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 22992310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkholder, R. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (2003). Speech timing and working memory in profoundly deaf children after cochlear implantation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 6388.Google Scholar
Campbell, R., Macsweeney, M., & Woll, B. (2014). Cochlear implantation (CI) for prelingual deafness: The relevance of studies of brain organization and the role of first language acquisition in considering outcome success. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8.Google Scholar
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Varuzza, C., Giuliani, A., & Burdo, S. (2012). Cochlear implant in the second year of life: Lexical and grammatical outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 382394.Google Scholar
Chamberlain, C., Morford, J. P., & Mayberry, R. I. (2000). Language acquisition by eye: Early language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cheung, H. (1996). Non-word span as a unique predictor of second language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867873.Google Scholar
Chiat, S., & Roy, P. (2007). The preschool repetition test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 429443.Google Scholar
Chilosi, A. M., Comparini, A., Scusa, M. F., Orazini, L., Forli, F., Cipriani, P., & Berrettini, S. (2013). A longitudinal study of lexical and grammar development in deaf Italian children provided with early cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 34, e28e37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N. (2004). Young children's sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 183213.Google Scholar
Cohen, N. J., Lojkasek, M., Zadeh, Z. Y., Pugliese, M., & Kiefer, H. (2008). Children adopted from China: A prospective study of their growth and development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 458468. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01853.x Google Scholar
Conway, C. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Kronenberger, W. G. (2009). The importance of sound for cognitive sequencing abilities the auditory scaffolding hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 275279.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., & Kail, R. (1996). Covert processes and their development in short-term memory. In Gathercole, S. E. (Ed.), Models of short-term memory (pp. 2950). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Wood, N. L., Wood, P. K., Keller, T. A., Nugent, L. D., & Keller, C. V. (1998). Two separate verbal processing rates contributing to short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 141160.Google Scholar
Cynader, M., & Mitchell, D. E. (1980). Prolonged sensitivity to monocular deprivation in dark-reared cats. Journal of Neurophysiology, 43, 10261040.Google Scholar
Dalen, M. (2002). School performances among internationally adopted children in Norway. Adoption Quarterly, 5, 3958. doi:10.1300/J145v05n0203 Google Scholar
DeGeer, B. (1992). Internationally adopted children in communication: A developmental study (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Lund University, Lund, Sweden).Google Scholar
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Spelke, E. S. (2015). The infancy of the human brain. Neuron, 88, 93109.Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A. (2016). Language, cognitive, and academic abilities of school-age internationally adopted children. In Genesee, F. & Delcenserie, A. (Eds.), Starting over—The language development of internationally-adopted children (pp. 95124). Trends in Language Acquisition Research series (TiLAR). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F. (2014a). Language and memory abilities of internationally-adopted children from China. Journal of Child Language, 41, 11951223. doi:10.1017/S030500091300041X Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F. (2014b). The acquisition of accusative object clitics by IA children from China: Do they still experience difficulties during school-age? Journal of Child Language, 42, 196209. doi:10.1017/S0305000913000500 Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F. (2014c). The performance of internationally-adopted children from China on sentence recall: A detailed analysis. In Orman, W. & Valleau, M. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 38th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 116128). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F. (2016). The effects of age of acquisition and bilingualism on verbal working memory. International Journal of Bilingualism. Advance online publication. doi:1177/1367006916639158 Google Scholar
Delcenserie, A., Genesee, F., & Gauthier, K. (2013). Language abilities of internationally-adopted (IA) children from China during the early school years: Evidence for early age effects? Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 541568. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000865 Google Scholar
Dunn, C. C., Walker, E. A., Oleson, J., Kenwothy, M., Van Voorst, T., Toblin, J. B., . . . Gantz, B. J. (2014). Longitudinal speech perception and language performance in pediatric cochlear implant users: The effect of age at implantation. Ear and Hearing, 35, 148160. doi:10.197/AUD.0b013e3182a4a8f0 Google Scholar
Durand, M., Hulme, C., Larkin, R., & Snowling, M. (2005). The cognitive foundations of reading and arithmetic skills in 7- to 10-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 113136.Google Scholar
Eigsti, I.-M., Weitzman, C., Schuh, J., De Marchena, A., & Casey, B. J. (2011). Language and cognitive outcomes in internationally adopted children. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 629646. doi:10.1017/S0954579411000204 Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Bellugi, U., Friederici, A., & Horn, P. (1995). Effects of age of acquisition on grammatical sensitivity: Evidence from online and offline tasks. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 123.Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P., Cruz-Santos, A., Tourinho, C., Martin, R., & Bialystok, E. (2012). Bilingualism enriches the poor: Enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23, 13641371. doi:10.1177/0956797612443836 Google Scholar
Engineer, N. D., Riley, J. R., Seale, J. D., Vrana, W. A., Shetake, J. A., Sudanagunta, S. P., . . . Kilgard, M. P. (2011). Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature, 470, 101104.Google Scholar
Fagiolini, M., Pizzorusso, T., Berardi, N., Domenici, L., & Maffei, L. (1994). Functional postnatal development of the rat primary visual cortex and the role of visual experience: Dark rearing and monocular deprivation. Vision Research, 34, 709720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fennell, C., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2014). You sound like mommy bilingual and monolingual infants learn words best from speakers typical of their language environments. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 309316.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 410418.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Adams, A. M. (1993). Phonological working memory in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 29, 770778.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Development of vocabulary in children and short-term phonological memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Hitch, G. J. (1993). Developmental changes in short-term memory: A revised working memory perspective. In Collins, A., Gathercole, S. E., Conway, M. A., & Morris, P. E. (Eds.), Theories of memory (pp. 189210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauthier, K., & Genesee, F. (2011). Language development in internationally-adopted children: A special case of early second language learning. Child Development, 82, 115. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01578.x Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., & Hayes, H. (2011). Reading, writing, and phonological processing skills of adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear and Hearing, 32, 49S59S.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., & Nicholas, J. G. (2013). Enduring advantages of early cochlear implantation for spoken language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 643655.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G., & Sedey, A. L. (2003). Language skills of children with early cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24, 46S58S.Google Scholar
Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J., Tobey, E., & Davidson, L. (2015). Persistent language delay versus late language emergence in children with early cochlear implantation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 155170.Google Scholar
Gervain, J. (2015). Plasticity in early language acquisition: The effects of prenatal and early childhood experience. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 35, 1320.Google Scholar
Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2008). How infant speech perception contributes to language acquisition. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 11491170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, D. A., Pratt, C., & Ong, B. (2005). The relations between children's reading comprehension, working memory, language skills, and components of reading decoding in a normal sample. Reading and Writing, 18, 583616.Google Scholar
Harris, M. S., Kronenberger, W. G., Gao, S., Hoen, H. M., Miyamoto, R. T., & Pisoni, D. B. (2013). Verbal short-term memory development and spoken language outcomes in deaf children with cochlear implants. Ear and Hearing, 34, 179192. doi:AUD.0b013e318269ce50 Google Scholar
Hensch, T. K. (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 877888.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.Google Scholar
Hough, S. D. (2005). Language outcomes in school-aged children adopted from Eastern European orphanages (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh).Google Scholar
Ingvalson, E. M., Holt, L. L., & McClelland, J. L. (2012). Can native Japanese listeners learn to differentiate/r–l/on the basis of F3 onset frequency? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 255274.Google Scholar
Ingvalson, E. M., McClelland, J. L., & Holt, L. L. (2011). Predicting native English-like performance by native Japanese speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 39, 571584.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.Google Scholar
Jones, F., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M. (2007). Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words. Developmental Science, 10, 853873.Google Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (2011). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44, 137166 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 123.Google Scholar
Kaliman, P., Alvarez-López, M. J., Cosín-Tomás, M., Rosenkranz, M. A., Lutz, A., & Davidson, R. J. (2014). Rapid changes in histone deacetylases and inflammatory gene expression in expert meditators. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 40, 96107. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.11.004 Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M. (2012). Cognitive mechanisms of word learning in bilingual and monolingual adults: The role of phonological memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 470489.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., & Buac, M. (2014). Effects of classroom bilingualism on task-shifting, verbal memory, and word learning in children. Developmental Science, 17, 564583.Google Scholar
Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M. M., DePaolis, R. A., Whitaker, C. J., & Williams, N. M. (2010). The role of vocal practice in constructing phonological working memory. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 12801293.Google Scholar
Kral, A., & Sharma, A. (2012). Developmental neuroplasticity after cochlear implantation. Trends in Neurosciences, 35, 111122.Google Scholar
Kramer, R., & Mota, M. B. (2015). Effects of bilingualism on inhibitory control and working memory: A study with early and late bilinguals. Gragoatá, 20, 309331.Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D. (1973). Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period: Some new evidence. Language Learning, 23, 6374.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., & Dufour, R. (2002). The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 18, 137171.Google Scholar
Kudo, M., & Swanson, H. L. (2014). Are there advantages for additive bilinguals in working memory tasks? Learning and Individual Differences, 35, 96102.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 1185011857. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850 Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831843.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron, 67, 713727.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K., & Linblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606608.Google Scholar
Kyle, F. E., & Cain, K. (2015). A comparison of deaf and hearing children's reading comprehension profiles. Topics in Language Disorders, 35, 144156.Google Scholar
Lany, J., & Saffran, J. R. (2013). Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy. In Rubenstein, J. L. R. & Rakic, P. (Eds.), Comprehensive developmental neuroscience: Neural circuit development and function in the brain (pp. 231248). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lederberg, A. R. (2006). Language development of deaf children with hearing parents. In Lieven, E. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (2nd ed., pp. 361367). Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Le Normand, M. T., Ouellet, C., & Cohen, H. (2003). Productivity of lexical categories in French-speaking children with cochlear implants. Brain and Cognition, 53, 257262.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21, 861883.Google Scholar
Lively, S. E., Pisoni, D. B., Yamada, R. A., Tohkura, Y. I., & Yamada, T. (1994). Training Japanese listeners to identify English/r/and/l/: III. Long-term retention of new phonetic categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96, 20762087.Google Scholar
Logan, J. S., Lively, S. E., & Pisoni, D. B. (1991). Training Japanese listeners to identify English /r/ and /l/: A first report. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 89, 874886.Google Scholar
Lu, J., Jones, A., & Morgan, G. (2016). The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. Journal of Child Language, 43, 537552. doi:10.1017/S0305000915000835 Google Scholar
Luk, G. (2015). Who are the bilinguals (and monolinguals)? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3536. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000625 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacSweeney, M., Waters, D., Brammer, M. J., Woll, B., & Goswami, U. (2008). Phonological processing in deaf signers and the impact of age of first language acquisition. NeuroImage, 40, 13691379. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.047 Google Scholar
Majerus, S., Amand, P., Boniver, V., Demanez, J.-P., Demanez, L., & van der Linden, M. (2005). A quantitative and qualitative assessment of verbal short-term memory and phonological processing in 8-year-olds with a history of repetitive otitis media. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 473498.Google Scholar
Marshall, C., Jones, A., Denmark, T., Mason, K., Atkinson, J., Botting, N., & Morgan, G. (2015). Deaf children's non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 112.Google Scholar
Mattock, K., Molnar, M., Polka, L., & Burnham, D. (2008). The developmental course of lexical tone perception in the first year of life. Cognition, 106, 13671381. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.002 Google Scholar
Mauer, D., & Werker, J. (2014). Perceptual narrowing during infancy: A comparison of language and faces. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 154178.Google Scholar
May, L., Byers-Heinlein, K., Gervain, J., & Werker, J. F. (2011). Language and the newborn brain: Does prenatal language experience shape the neonate neural response to speech? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 222.Google Scholar
Maya Vetencourt, J. F., Sale, A., Viegi, A., Baroncelli, L., De Pasquale, R., O'Leary, O. F., . . . Maffei, L. (2008). The antidepressant fluoxetine restores plasticity in the adult visual cortex. Science, 320, 385388. doi:10.1126/science.1150516 Google Scholar
Maya Vetencourt, J. F., Tiraboschi, E., Spolidoro, M., Castrén, E., & Maffei, L. (2011). Serotonin triggers a transient epigenetic mechanism that reinstates adult visual cortex plasticity in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 4957. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07488.x Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (1993). First-language acquisition after childhood differs from second-language acquisition: The case of American Sign Language. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 5168.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. (2007). When timing is everything: Age of first-language acquisition effects on second-language learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 537.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I., & Lock, E. (2003). Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity and epigenesis. Brain and Language, 87, 369383.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101B111.Google Scholar
McCandliss, B. D., Fiez, J. A., Protopapas, A., Conway, M., & McClelland, J. L. (2002). Success and failure in teaching the [r]-[l] contrast to Japanese adults: Tests of a Hebbian model of plasticity and stabilization in spoken language perception. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 89108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y. (2010). Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 306323.Google Scholar
Miller, E. M., Lederberg, A. R., & Easterbrooks, S. R. (2013). Phonological awareness: Explicit instruction for young deaf and hard-of-hearing children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18, 206227.Google Scholar
Mody, M., Schwartz, R. G., Gravel, J. S., & Ruben, R. J. (1999). Speech perception and verbal memory in children with and without histories and otitis media. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 10691079.Google Scholar
Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1985). Electrophysiological indices of auditory discrimination in newborn infants: The bases for predicting later language development? Infant Behavior and Development, 8, 197211.Google Scholar
Morford, J. P. (2002). Why does exposure to language matter? In Givoń, T. & Malle, B. (Eds.), The evolution of language from pre-language (pp. 329341). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Morgan, G. (2014). Critical period in language development. In Brookes, P. & Kempe, V. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language development (pp. 115118). Los Angeles: Sage.Google Scholar
Morgan, G. (2015). On language acquisition in speech and sign: Development of combinatorial structure in both modalities. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 5, 1217.Google Scholar
Morgan, G., & Woll, B. (2002). Directions in sign language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Morton, J. B. (2014). Sunny review casts a foreboding shadow over status quo bilingual advantage research. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 929931.Google Scholar
Mower, G. D. (1991). Comparison of serotonin 5-HT1 receptors and innervation in the visual cortex of normal and dark-reared cats. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 312, 223230.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. (1995). The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Psychology, 31, 723738.Google Scholar
Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 117.Google Scholar
Niparko, J. K., Tobey, E. A., Thal, D. J., Eisenberg, L. S., Wang, N. Y., Quittner, A. L., . . . CDaCI Investigative Team. (2010). Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. Journal of the American Medical Association, 303, 14981506.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., & Burton, L.T. (2005). The role of early language experience in the development of speech perception and phonological processing abilities: Evidence from 5-year-olds with histories of otitis media with effusion and low socioeconomic status. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38, 2963.Google Scholar
Nittrouer, S., Lowenstein, J. H., & Holloman, C. (2016). Early predictors of phonological and morphosyntactic skills in second graders with cochlear implants. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 55, 143160.Google Scholar
Orfanidou, E., Adam, R., Morgan, G., & McQueen, J. M. (2010). Segmentation in signed and spoken language: Different modalities, same segmentation procedures. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 272283.Google Scholar
Pallier, C., Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (1997). A limit on behavioral plasticity in speech perception. Cognition, 64, B9–B17.Google Scholar
Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2010). Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing 2-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 113125.Google Scholar
Pearce, P. S., Saunders, M. A., Creighton, D. E., & Sauve, R. S. (1988). Hearing and verbal-cognitive abilities in high-risk preterm infants prone to otitis media with effusion. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 9, 346351.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., Dubins, M. H., Jasinska, K., & Shalinsky, M. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121, 130143.Google Scholar
Philpot, B. D., Sekhar, A. K., Shouval, H. Z., & Bear, M. F. (2001). Visual experience and deprivation bidirectionally modify the composition and function of NMDA receptors in visual cortex. Neuron, 29, 157169.Google Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Chen, J. K., Delcenserie, A., Genesee, F., & Klein, D. (2015). Past experience shapes ongoing neural patterns for language. Nature Communications, 6.Google Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Klein, D., Chen, J. K., Delcenserie, A., & Genesee, F. (2014). Mapping the unconscious maintenance of a lost first language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 1731417319.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., & Cleary, M. (2003). Measures of working memory span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 24, 106S.Google Scholar
Pisoni, D. B., Kronenberger, W., Roman, A., & Geers, A. (2011). Measures of digit span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children following more than 10 years of cochlear implantation. Ear and Hearing, 32 (Suppl. 1), 60S–74SGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., & Werker, J. F. (1994). Developmental changes in perception of nonnative vowel contrasts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 421435.Google Scholar
Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., Chicoine, J. F., Séguin, R., Belhumeur, C., Germain, P., . . . Jéliu, G. (2005). Health status, cognitive and motor development of young children adopted from China, East Asia, and Russia across the first 6 months after adoption. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 445457.Google Scholar
Ratiu, I., & Azuma, T. (2015). Working memory capacity: Is there a bilingual advantage? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27, 111.Google Scholar
Repovš, G., & Baddeley, A. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: Explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience, 139, 521.Google Scholar
Rhine-Kahlbeck, S. (2004). The assessment of developmental language differences, executive functioning, and social skills in deaf children (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Gallaudet University).Google Scholar
Roberts, G., Quach, J., Mensah, F., Gathercole, S., Gold, L., Anderson, P., . . . Wake, M. (2015). Schooling duration rather than chronological age predicts working memory between 6 and 7 years: Memory Maestros study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 36, 6874.Google Scholar
Roberts, J. E., Long, S. H., Malkin, C., Barnes, E., Skinner, M., & Hennon, E. A. (2005). A comparison of phonological skills of boys with fragile X syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 980995.Google Scholar
Ruben, R. J. (1997). A time frame of critical/sensitive periods of language development. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 117, 202205.Google Scholar
Sarant, J., Harris, D., Bennet, L., & Bant, S. (2014). Bilateral versus unilateral cochlear implants in children: A study of spoken language outcomes. Ear and Hearing, 35, 396409.Google Scholar
Schick, B., Marschark, M., & Spencer, P. (Eds.) (2005). Advances in the sign language development of deaf children. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scott, K. A. (2009). Language outcomes of school-age internationally-adopted children: A systematic review of the literature. Topics in Language Disorders, 29, 6581. doi:10.1097/TLD.0b013e3181977b4c Google Scholar
Scott, K. A., & Roberts, J. A. (2016). Language development during the preschool years. In Genesee, F. & Delcenserie, A. (Eds.), Starting over—The language development of internationally-adopted children (pp. 6594). Trends in Language Acquisition Research series (TiLAR). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Scott, K. A., Roberts, J. A., & Krakow, R. (2008). Oral and written language development of children adopted from China. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 150160.Google Scholar
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 2150.Google Scholar
Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155172.Google Scholar
Service, E., Simola, M., Metsänheimo, O., & Maury, S. (2002). Bilingual working memory span is affected by language skill. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14, 383408.Google Scholar
Silingardi, D., Scali, M., Belluomini, G., & Pizzorusso, T. (2010). Epigenetic treatments of adult rats promote recovery from visual acuity deficits induced by long-term monocular deprivation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 21852192.Google Scholar
Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2011). Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 112.Google Scholar
Soleymani, Z., Amidfar, M., Dadgar, H., & Jalaie, S. (2014). Working memory in Farsi-speaking children with normal development and cochlear implant. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 78, 674678.Google Scholar
Spencer, L. J., & Tomblin, J. B. (2009). Evaluating phonological processing skills in children with prelingual deafness who use cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14, 121.Google Scholar
Stager, C. L., & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature, 388, 381382.Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13, 480484.Google Scholar
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., . . . Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 8184.Google Scholar
Tan, T. X. (2009). School-age adopted Chinese girls’ behavior adjustment, academic performance, and social skills: Longitudinal results. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79, 244251. doi:10.1037/a0015682 Google Scholar
Thorn, A. S. C., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 52, 303324.Google Scholar
Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A. M., Laakso, M. L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Predicting delayed letter knowledge development and its relation to Grade 1 reading achievement among children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 42, 1128.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75, 10671084.Google Scholar
Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399433.Google Scholar
van Daal, J., Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & van Balkom, H. (2008). Working memory limitations in children with severe language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 85107.Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M., Juffer, F., & Poelhuis, C. (2005). Adoption and cognitive development: A metaanalytic comparison of adopted and non-adopted children's IQ and school performance. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 301316. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.301 Google Scholar
Vejnović, D., Milin, P., & Zdravković, S. (2010). Effects of proficiency and age of language acquisition on working memory performance in bilinguals. Psihologija, 43, 219232.Google Scholar
Wake, M., Poulakis, Z., Hugues, E. K., Carey-Sargeant, C., & Rickards, F. W. (2005). Hearing impairment: A population study of age at diagnosis, severity, and language outcomes at 7–8 years. Archives of Disease Childhood, 90, 238244.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Fennell, C. T. (2009). Bilingual beginnings to learning words. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364, 36493663.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants' ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3, 130.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, K., & Tees, R. C. (1981). Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. Child Development, 52, 349355.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Hensch, T. K. (2015). Critical periods in speech perception: New directions. Psychology, 66, 173.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 4963.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005). Speech perception as a window for understanding plasticity and commitment in language systems of the brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 233251.Google Scholar
Whitton, J. P., & Polley, D. B. (2011). Evaluating the perceptual and pathophysiological consequences of auditory deprivation in early postnatal life: A comparison of basic and clinical studies. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, 12, 535547.Google Scholar
Yoshida, K. A., Pons, F., Maye, J., & Werker, J. F. (2010). Distributional phonetic learning at 10 months of age. Infancy, 15, 420433.Google Scholar
Yoo, J., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2012). Phonological memory in bilinguals and monolinguals. Memory and Cognition, 40, 13141330.Google Scholar