Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T14:55:54.777Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Jumping around and leaving things out: A profile of the narrative abilities of children with specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

A. Elisabeth Miranda
Affiliation:
Federal University of Cerea
Allyssa McCabe*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Lowell
Lynn S. Bliss
Affiliation:
University of Houston
*
Allyssa McCabe, Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts-Lowell, One University Avenue, Lowell, MA 01854

Abstract

This article investigates the discourse coherence of school-aged children with specific language impairment (SLI). The following dimensions of discourse are analyzed: topic maintenance, event sequencing, explicitness (including referencing), conjunctive cohesion, and fluency. The personal narratives of the children in the experimental group were compared with those produced by two groups of children with normal language development, one group matched by chronological age and the other matched by language level. The narratives of the children with SLI were significantly impaired compared with both control groups with respect to all five dimensions of narration, although impairment was far more pronounced for topic maintenance, event sequencing, and implicitness than it was for conjunctive cohesion or fluency. The former serious impairments place a heavy burden on listeners. Theoretical and clinical implications of the results are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bamberg, M. (1987). A functional approach to the acquisition of anaphoric relationships. Linguistics, 24, 227284.Google Scholar
Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (1994). Narrative structure. In Berman, R. & Slobin, D. (Eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study (pp. 3984). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bliss, L. S. (1992). A comparison of tactful messages by children with and without language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 23, 343347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bliss, L. S. (1993). Pragmatic language intervention: Interactive activities. Eau Claire, WI: Thinking Publications.Google Scholar
Bliss, L. S., & Allen, D. V. (1983). Screening kit of language development. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Bliss, L. S., McCabe, A., & Miranda, A. E. (1996). Assessment of narrative skills of schoolage children. Unpublished manuscript, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI.Google Scholar
Craig, H. (1991). Pragmatic characteristics of the child with specific language impairment: An interactionist perspective. In Gallagher, T. M. (Ed.), Pragmatics of language clinical practice issues (pp. 163198). San Diego: Singular Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasinger, L., & Toupin, C. (1994). The development of relative clause functions in narrative. In Berman, R. & Slobin, D. (Eds.). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study (pp. 457514). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Deese, J. (1984). Thought into speech: The psychology of a language. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
DiSimoni, F. (1978). The Token Test for Children. Hingham, MA: Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
Feagans, L., & Short, E. (1984). Developmental differences in the comprehension and production of narratives in reading-disabled and normally achieving children. Child Development, 55, 17271736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, M. F. (1979). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
German, D. J. (1987). Spontaneous language profiles of children with word-finding problems. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 18, 217230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
German, D. J., & Simon, S. (1991). Analysis of children's word-finding skills in discourse. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 309316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybeal, C. (1981). Memory for stories in language-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2. 269283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K., & Hasan, R. (1976). Cohesion in English. London: Longman Group.Google Scholar
Hammill, D., & Newcomer, P. (1982). Test of Language Development - Primary Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Hemphill, L. (1989). Topic development, syntax, and social class. Discourse Processes, 12, 267286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1992). How should input be measured? First Language, 12, 233244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ito, T. (1986). Speech dysfluencies and acquisition of syntax in children 2–6 years old. Folia Phoniatica, 38, 310.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1982). Narratives: A new look at communication problems in older language impaired children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 13, 144155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. (1989). Specific language disorders in the child. In Lass, N., McReynolds, L., Northern, J., & Yoder, D. (Eds.), Speech, language, and hearing (pp. 685715). Philadelphia: Saunders.Google Scholar
Johnston, J. (1991). Questions about cognition in children with specific language impairment. In Miller, J. (Ed.), Research on child language disorders: A decade of progress (pp. 299308). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Liles, B. Z. (1985a). Cohesion in the narratives of normal and language disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liles, B. Z. (1985b). The production and comprehension of narrative discourse in normal and language disordered children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 18, 409427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liles, B. Z. (1987). Episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children with and without language disorder. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 185196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liles, B. Z. (1993). Narrative discourse in children with language disorders and children with normal language: A critical review of the literature. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 868882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLachlan, B., & Chapman, R. (1988). Communication breakdowns in normal and language learning-disabled children's conversation and narration. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B., & Snow, C. E. (1985). The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language, 12, 271295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, A. (1996). Chameleon readers: Teaching children to appreciate all kinds of good stories. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
McCabe, A., & Rollins, P. R. (1994). Assessment of preschool narrative skills. Prerequisite for|literacy. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: A Journal of Clinical Practice, 13, 4556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGann, W., & Schwartz, A. (1988). Main character in children's narratives. Linguistics, 26, 215233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merritt, D. D., & Liles, B. Z. (1987). Story grammar ability in children with and without language disorder: Story generation, story retelling, and story comprehension. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 30, 539552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miranda, A. E., McCabe, A., & Bliss, L. S. (1994). Jumping around and leaving things out: Assessing impaired narration. Miniseminar presented at the annual convention of the American Speech-Language Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Olley, L. (1989). Oral narrative of normal and language-impaired school aged children. Australian Journal of Human Communication Disorders, 17, 4365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1991). On the threshold of the storyrealm: Semantic versus pragmatic use of connectives in narratives. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 445464.Google Scholar
Purcell, S. L., & Liles, B. Z. (1992). Cohesion repairs in the narratives of normal-language and language disordered school aged children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 354362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, R., & Spekman, N. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spontaneously generated stories of learning-disabled and normally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51, 823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sabin, E. J., Clemmer, E. J., O'Connell, D. C., & Kowal, S. (1979). A pausological approach to speech development. In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (Eds.), Of speech and time: Temporal speech patterns in interpersonal context (pp. 742755). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Index of Productive Syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sleight, C., & Printz, P. M. (1985). Use of abstracts, orientations, and codas in narration by language-disordered and nondisordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 50, 361371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar