Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T16:54:23.894Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychiatric disorders and the social brain: Distinguishing mentalizing and empathizing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 June 2008

Alfonso Troisi
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, University of Rome, Tor Vergata, 00161 Rome, Italy. alfonso.troisi@uniroma2.it

Abstract

Social cognition is a broad term, incorporating all aspects of social functioning from perceiving emotional stimuli to attributional style and theory of mind. Not distinguishing between these different capacities may confound the interpretation of the data deriving from studies of the relationship between psychiatric disorders and the social brain. The distinction between cognitive and affective components of social cognition is clearly exemplified by the abnormalities observed in psychopathy and Williams syndrome.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Akel, A. & Abushua'leh, K. (2004) “Theory of mind” in violent and nonviolent patients with paranoid schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 69:4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2003) Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry 182:57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brüne, M., Ribbert, H. & Schiefenhövel, W. (2003) The social brain: Evolution and pathology. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, J. K. (2006b) The social brain hypothesis of schizophrenia. World Psychiatry 5:7781.Google ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, R. & Frith, C. D. (1996) Conversational conduct and the symptoms of schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 1:305–18.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2004) Schizophrenia and theory of mind. Psychological Medicine 34:385–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langdon, R., Coltheart, M., Ward, P. B. & Catts, S. (2002) Disturbed communication in schizophrenia: The role of poor pragmatics and poor mind-reading. Psychological Medicine 32:1273–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, R., Leudar, I. & Antaki, C. (2004) Do people with schizophrenia display theory of mind deficits in clinical interactions? Psychological Medicine 34:401–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Mervis, C. B. & Berman, K. F. (2006) Neural mechanisms in Williams syndrome: A unique window to genetic influences on cognition and behaviour. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 7:380–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S. & Blair, R. J. (2003) Theory of mind and psychopathy: Can psychopathic individuals read the “language of the eyes”? Neuropsychologia 41:523–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Shur, S., Barcai-Goodman, L., Medlovich, S., Harari, H. & Levkovitz, Y. (2007) Dissociation of cognitive from affective components of theory of mind in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 149:1123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T. (2006) The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30:855–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troisi, A. (2005) The concept of alternative strategies and its relevance to psychiatry and clinical psychology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29:159–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed