Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T09:40:49.677Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Young children's uptake of new words in conversation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2007

EVE V. CLARK
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Margaret Jacks Hall, Bldg 460, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305-2150, eclark@psych.stanford.edu

Abstract

When offered unfamiliar words, do children attend to them? Examination of 701 offers of new words drawn from the longitudinal records of five children provides extensive evidence of attention to the new words: Children repeated the new word in the next turn 54% of the time; they acknowledged it in the next turn with markers like yeah or uh-huh 9% of the time, or made a relevant move-on by alluding to some aspect of its referent, again in the next turn, 38% of the time. By comparison, the repeat-rate in new-to-given shifts in conversation is significantly lower. The present data provide strong evidence for some immediate uptake. When children register that new words are new, they can assign them some preliminary meaning and begin to use them right away from as young as age two.This research was supported in part by grants from the National Science Foundation (SBR97-31781), the Spencer Foundation (199900133), and the Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford University. I am indebted to Andrew D-W. Wong for help with the data analysis, to Karin Kastens for locating elusive articles, and to Bruno Estigarribia, Barbara F. Kelly, David A. McKercher, and Edy Veneziano for discussion and comment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, A. Karen, & Bullock, Dan (1986). Apprenticeship in word use: Social convergence processes in learning categorically related nouns. In Stanley A. Kuczaj & Martyn D. Barrett (eds.), The development of word meaning, 15597. New York: Springer.CrossRef
Akhtar, Nameera; Dunham, Frances; & Dunham, Philip J. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language 18:4149.Google Scholar
Anglin, Jeremy M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58 (Serial No. 238).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bangerter, Adrian (2004). Using pointing and describing to achieve joint focus of attention in dialogue. Psychological Science 15:41519.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon; Baldwin, Dare A.; & Crowson, Mary (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development 68:4857.Google Scholar
Bloom, Lois; Hood, Lois; & Lightbown, Patsy M. (1974). Imitation in language development: If, when, and why. Cognitive Development 6:380420.Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa (2005). Why can't you ‘open’ a nut or ‘break’ a cooked noodle? Learning covert object categories in action word meanings. In Lisa Gershkoff-Stowe & David H. Rakison (eds.), Building object categories in developmental time, 20943. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Brennan, Susan E. (1996). Lexical entrainment in spontaneous dialog. Proceedings, 1996 International Symposium on Spoken Dialogue, 4144. Philadelphia: ISSD-96.
Brown, Roger W. (1957). Linguistic determinism and the part of speech. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology 55:15.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger W. (1958). Words and things. New York: Free Press.
Brown, Roger W. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Brown, Roger W., & Bellugi, Ursula. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34:13351.Google Scholar
Callanan, Maureen A. (1989). Maternal speech strategies and children's acquisition of hierarchical category labels. Genetic Psychology 17:312.Google Scholar
Callanan, Maureen A. (1991). Parent-child collaboration in young children's understanding of category hierarchies. In Susan A. Gelman & James P. Byrnes (eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development, 44084. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Callanan, Maureen A., & Sabbagh, Mark A. (2004). Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents' language and children's developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology 40:74663.Google Scholar
Carey, Susan (1978). The child as word learner. In Morris Halle et al. (eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality, 26493. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Carey, Susan, & Bartlett, Elsa (1978). Acquiring a single new word. In Eve V. Clark (ed.), Papers and Reports on Child Language Development [Stanford University] 15:1729.
Chouinard, Michelle M., & Clark, Eve V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language 30:63769.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Clark, Eve V. (1997). Conceptual perspective and lexical choice in acquisition. Cognition 64:137.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. (1998). Lexical structure and pragmatic directions in acquisition. In M. Catherine Gruber et al. (eds.), Papers from the 34th meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society 1998: The Panels, 43746. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Clark, Eve V. (2001). Grounding and attention in the acquisition of language. In Mary Andronis et al. (eds.), Papers from the 37th meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, vol. 1, 95116. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Clark, Eve V. (2002). Making use of pragmatic inferences in the acquisition of meaning. In David Beaver et al. (eds.), The construction of meaning, 4558. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
Clark, Eve V. (2005a, June). Conventionality and semantic domains. Paper presented at NSF Workshop on “Words and the world: how words capture human experience,” Lehigh University, PA.
Clark, Eve V. (2005b, September). A gradualist view of meaning. Paper presented at Workshop on “Word meaning, concepts, and communication,” Cumberland Lodge, Windsor Great Park, UK.
Clark, Eve V., & Estigarribia, Bruno (2005, July). Getting and maintaining attention in conversations with young children. Paper presented at Xth International Congress of the Association for the Study of Child Language (IASCL), Freie Universität-Berlin.
Clark, Eve V., & Grossman, James B. (1998). Pragmatic directions and children's word learning. Journal of Child Language 25:118.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Svaib, Trisha A. (1997). Speaker perspective and reference in young children. First Language 17:5774.Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V., & Wong, Andrew D-W. (2002). Pragmatic directions about language use: words and word meanings. Language in Society 31:181212.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Clark, Herbert H., & Schaefer, Edward F. (1989). Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science 13:25994.Google Scholar
Deák, Gedeon O., & Maratsos, Michael P. (1998). On having complex representation of things: Preschoolers use multiple words for objects and people. Developmental Psychology 34:22440.Google Scholar
Diesendruck, Gil, & Shatz, Marilyn (1997). The effect of perceptual similarity and linguistic input on children's acquisition of object labels. Journal of Child Language 24:695717.Google Scholar
Dockrell, Julie (1981). The child's acquisition of unfamiliar words: An experimental study. Dissertation, University of Stirling, UK.
Dockrell, Julie, & McShane, John. (1990). Young children's use of phrase structure and inflectional information in form-class assignment of novel nouns and verbs. First Language 10:12740.Google Scholar
Ervin, Susan (1964). Imitation and structural change in children's language. In Eric H. Lenneberg (ed.), New directions in the study of language, 16389. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Estigarribia, Bruno, & Clark, Eve V. (2006). Getting and maintaining attention in talk to young children. Ms., Stanford University, September 2006.
Gleitman, Lila R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1:355.Google Scholar
Goodwin, Charles (1981). Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic Press.
Goodwin, Charles (2001). Dissecting common ground: Examining an instance of reference repair. In Joanna D. Moore & Keith Stenning (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 51621. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Hall, D. Geoffrey; Lee, Sharon C.; & Bélanger, Julie (2001). Young children's use of syntactic cues to learn proper names and count nouns. Developmental Psychology 27:298307.Google Scholar
Heibeck, Tracy H., & Markman, Ellen M. (1987). Word learning in children: an examination of fast mapping. Child Development 58:102134.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, Stanley A., II (1977). The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 4:395416.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian, & Snow, Catherine. (1985). The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language 12:27196.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian, & Snow, Catherine (1990). The Child Language Data Exchange System: An update. Journal of Child Language 17:45772.Google Scholar
Malt, Barbara C.; Sloman, Steven A.; & Gennari, Silvia P. (2003). Universality and language specificity in object naming. Journal of Memory & Language 49:2042.Google Scholar
Malt, Barbara C.; Sloman, Steven A.; Gennari, Silvia P.; Shi, Meiyi; & Wang, Yuan (1999). Knowing versus naming: Similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifacts. Journal of Memory & Language 40:23062.Google Scholar
Masur, Elise F., & Gleason, Jean B. (1980). Parent-child interaction and the acquisition of lexical information during play. Developmental Psychology 16:4049.Google Scholar
McTear, Michael (1978). Repetition in child language: Imitation or creation? In Robin N. Campbell & Philip T. Smith (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language, 293311. London: Plenum.
McTear, Michael (1985). Children's conversation. Oxford: Blackwell.
Merritt, Marilyn (1994). Repetition in situated discourse: Exploring its forms and functions. In Barbara Johnstone (ed.), Repetition in discourse: Interdisciplinary perspectives, vol. 1, 2336. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Ochs, Elinor (1977) Making it last: Repetition in children's discourse. In Susan Ervin-Tripp & Claudia Mitchell (eds.), Child discourse, 12538. New York: Academic Press.
Painter, Claire (1984). Into the mother tongue: A case study. London: Pinter.
Pye, Clifton; Loeb, Diane Frome; & Pao, Yin-yin (1995). The acquisition of breaking and cutting. In Eve V. Clark (ed.), Proceedings of the 27th annual child language research forum, 22736. Stanford, CA: CSLI.
Réger, Zita (1986). The functions of imitation in child language. Applied Psycholinguistics 7:32352.Google Scholar
Rogers, Don (1978). Information about word-meanings in the speech of parents to young children. In Robin N. Campbell & Philip T. Smith (eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language: Language development and mother-child interaction, 18798. New York: Plenum.
Sachs, Jacqueline (1979). Topic selection in parent-child discourse. Discourse Processes 2:14553.Google Scholar
Schegloff, Emanuel A. (1997). Practices and actions: Boundary cases of other-initiated repair. Discourse Processes 23:499545.Google Scholar
Schober, Michael F. (1993). Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition 47:124.Google Scholar
Shipley, Elizabeth F., & Kuhn, Ivy F. (1983). A constraint on comparisons: Equally detailed alternatives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35:195222.Google Scholar
Shipley, Elizabeth F.; Kuhn, Ivy F.; & Madden, E. Colby (1983). Mothers' use of superordinate category terms. Journal of Child Language 10:57188.Google Scholar
Speidel, Gisela E., & Nelson, Keith E. (eds.) (1989). The many faces of imitation in language learning. Berlin: Springer.CrossRef
Strapp, Chehalis M., & Federico, Amy (2000). Imitations and repetitions: What do children say following recasts? First Language 20:27390.Google Scholar
Svennevig, Jan (2004). Other-repetition as display of hearing, understanding and emotional stance. Discourse Studies 6:489516.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Chris Moore & Philip J. Dunham (eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, 10341. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Tomasello, Michael, & Farrar, Michael J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57:145463.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, & Haberl, Katharina (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology 39:90612.Google Scholar
Veneziano, Edy; Sinclair, Hermine; & Berthoud, Iohanna (1990). From one word to two words: Repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language 17:633650.Google Scholar
Walker, Marilyn A. (1996). Inferring acceptance and rejection in dialog by default rules of inference. Language & Speech 39:265304.Google Scholar
Waxman, Sandra R., & Senghas, Anne (1992). Relations among word meanings in early lexical development. Developmental Psychology 28:86273.Google Scholar
Waxman, Sandra R.; Shipley, Elizabeth F.; & Shepperson, Barbara (1991). Establishing new subcategories: The role of category labels and existing knowledge. Child Development 62:12738.Google Scholar
Wilkins, David (2002). On being “open”: An ethno-semantic description of the English verb ‘open’ based on adult-child interactions. Paper presented at the Symposium in Honor of Melissa Bowerman, MPI for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen.
Woodward, Amanda L. (2003). Infants' developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science 6:297311.Google Scholar