Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T07:53:01.199Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement remediates anhedonia in chronic opioid use by enhancing neurophysiological responses during savoring of natural rewards

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2021

Eric L. Garland*
Affiliation:
Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA College of Social Work, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Veterans Health Care Administration VISN 19 Whole Health Flagship site located at the VA Salt Lake City Health Care System, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Spencer T. Fix
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland
Justin P. Hudak
Affiliation:
Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA College of Social Work, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Edward M. Bernat
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland
Yoshio Nakamura
Affiliation:
Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Department of Anesthesiology, Division of Pain Medicine, Pain Research Center, University of Utah School of Medicine
Adam W. Hanley
Affiliation:
Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Health Intervention Development, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA College of Social Work, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
Gary W. Donaldson
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology, Division of Pain Medicine, Pain Research Center, University of Utah School of Medicine
William R. Marchand
Affiliation:
Veterans Health Care Administration VISN 19 Whole Health Flagship site located at the VA Salt Lake City Health Care System, Salt Lake City, UT, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Utah School of Medicine
Brett Froeliger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, University of Missouri
*
Author for correspondence: Eric L. Garland, E-mail: eric.garland@socwk.utah.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Neuropsychopharmacologic effects of long-term opioid therapy (LTOT) in the context of chronic pain may result in subjective anhedonia coupled with decreased attention to natural rewards. Yet, there are no known efficacious treatments for anhedonia and reward deficits associated with chronic opioid use. Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE), a novel behavioral intervention combining training in mindfulness with savoring of natural rewards, may hold promise for treating anhedonia in LTOT.

Methods

Veterans receiving LTOT (N = 63) for chronic pain were randomized to 8 weeks of MORE or a supportive group (SG) psychotherapy control. Before and after the 8-week treatment groups, we assessed the effects of MORE on the late positive potential (LPP) of the electroencephalogram and skin conductance level (SCL) during viewing and up-regulating responses (i.e. savoring) to natural reward cues. We then examined whether these neurophysiological effects were associated with reductions in subjective anhedonia by 4-month follow-up.

Results

Patients treated with MORE demonstrated significantly increased LPP and SCL to natural reward cues and greater decreases in subjective anhedonia relative to those in the SG. The effect of MORE on reducing anhedonia was statistically mediated by increases in LPP response during savoring.

Conclusions

MORE enhances motivated attention to natural reward cues among chronic pain patients on LTOT, as evidenced by increased electrocortical and sympathetic nervous system responses. Given neurophysiological evidence of clinical target engagement, MORE may be an efficacious treatment for anhedonia among chronic opioid users, people with chronic pain, and those at risk for opioid use disorder.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Patients with chronic non-cancer pain are commonly treated with long-term opioid therapy (LTOT; ⩾90 days of opioid use) (Chou et al., Reference Chou, Fanciullo, Fine, Adler, Ballantyne and Davies2009) despite risks including opioid misuse and opioid use disorder (OUD) (Chou et al., Reference Chou, Turner, Devine, Hansen, Sullivan, Blazina and Deyo2015). Although many patients take opioids as prescribed by their physician, approximately 25% of individuals receiving LTOT for pain engage in opioid misusing behaviors (Vowles et al., Reference Vowles, McEntee, Julnes, Frohe, Ney and van der Goes2015) such as unauthorized dose escalation or using opioids to alleviate dysphoria (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Budman, Fernandez, Houle, Benoit, Katz and Jamison2007). Hence, the drive to alleviate dysphoria with opioids is a key risk factor in the development of opioid misuse and OUD. Concomitantly, anhedonia, a reduced capacity to pursue, experience, and/or learn from pleasure (Rømer Thomsen, Whybrow, & Kringelbach, Reference Rømer Thomsen, Whybrow and Kringelbach2015), is a core feature of chronic pain that is magnified by opioid misuse (Garland, Trøstheim, Eikemo, Ernst, & Leknes, Reference Garland, Trøstheim, Eikemo, Ernst and Leknes2020; Trøstheim et al., Reference Trøstheim, Eikemo, Meir, Hansen, Paul, Kroll and Leknes2020)

Exogenous opioids interact with endogenous opioid and dopamine systems (Johnson & North, Reference Johnson and North1992; Spagnolo et al., Reference Spagnolo, Kimes, Schwandt, Shokri-Kojori, Thada, Phillips and Heilig2019) involved in regulating pain and reward. Over time, chronic opioid use in the context of prolonged pain is thought to produce anhedonia and blunt reward processing by dysregulating dopaminergic and opioidergic mechanisms underpinning healthy hedonic functioning (Volkow & McLellan, Reference Volkow and McLellan2016). High dose use and misuse of opioid analgesics is thereby theorized to exacerbate pain-related hedonic dysregulation by (a) producing neuroadaptations in corticostriatal reward systems and (b) by magnifying antireward processes instantiated by limbic systems (e.g. extended amygdala) that mediate the release of signaling molecules including corticotropin-releasing factor, dynorphin, and substance p (Garland, Froeliger, Zeidan, Partin, & Howard, Reference Garland, Froeliger, Zeidan, Partin and Howard2013; Koob, Reference Koob2020). The consequent allostatic shift in brain reward threshold is thought to lead to anhedonia and dysphoria, compelling opioid dose escalation as a means of obtaining hedonic equilibrium. Insofar as anhedonia is often coupled with decreased allocation of attention to positive stimuli and reward-predicting cues (Armstrong & Olatunji, Reference Armstrong and Olatunji2012; Brailean, Koster, Hoorelbeke, & De Raedt, Reference Brailean, Koster, Hoorelbeke and De Raedt2014; Huhn et al., Reference Huhn, Brooner, Sweeney, Antoine, Hammond, Ayaz and Dunn2021; Rømer Thomsen, Reference Rømer Thomsen2015), opioid misusing chronic pain patients demonstrate reduced autonomic and attentional responses to natural reward cues (Garland, Froeliger, & Howard, Reference Garland, Froeliger and Howard2015a, Reference Garland, Bryan, Nakamura, Froeliger and Howard2017c) relative to medication-adherent pain patients and healthy controls. Patients with OUD exhibit a similar attenuation of event-related potentials (ERP) of the electroencephalogram (EEG) during attention to natural reward cues, and this impairment predicts the risk of relapse (Lubman et al., Reference Lubman, Yucel, Kettle, Scaffidi, Mackenzie, Simmons and Allen2009). Specifically, the late positive potential (LPP) of the EEG indexes motivated attention to emotionally salient stimuli, which elicit a larger LPP than neutral stimuli reaching maximum amplitude at parietal sites (Pz) between 400 and 2000 ms after image onset (Hajcak & Foti, Reference Hajcak and Foti2020; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000). Beyond deficits in ‘bottom-up’ attention to natural reward, chronic pain patients who engage in high-dose opioid use and misuse also exhibit an inability to use ‘top-down’ cognitive control to up-regulate sympathetic responses to natural reward cues, as evidenced by blunted skin conductance levels (SCL) as early as several seconds after stimulus onset (Hudak et al., Reference Hudak, Prince, Marchand, Nakamura, Hanley, Bryan and Garland2021). SCL has been shown to index attention to emotionally salient stimuli (Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert, & Lang, Reference Bradley, Codispoti, Cuthbert and Lang2001) and to covary with the LPP and arousal ratings of images representing natural rewards (Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, Reference Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer and Lang2000). Further, the anticipation of reward results in increases in SCL that can be modulated by emotion regulation strategies – with concomitant effects on corticostriatal activity (Delgado, Gillis, & Phelps, Reference Delgado, Gillis and Phelps2008; Smith, Rigney, & Delgado, Reference Smith, Rigney and Delgado2016). Although SCL is a non-specific biomarker that can also index the orienting reflex (Sokolov, Reference Sokolov1963) and conditioning responses (Oe et al., Reference Oe, Schumacher, Schnyder, Mueller-Pfeiffer, Wilhelm, Kuelen and Martin-Soelch2016), attenuated SCL observed when people on LTOT attempt to up-regulate their response to natural reward might be driven by dysfunctional corticostriatal mechanisms and indicative of impaired emotional attention and arousal. Given this interpretation, neurophysiological deficits during attention to natural reward cues may contribute to symptoms of anhedonia and deficient reward processing in people with chronic pain on LTOT.

To date, there are no known efficacious treatments for anhedonia and concomitant deficits in attention to natural reward associated with chronic opioid use (Kiluk, Yip, DeVito, Carroll, & Sofuoglu, Reference Kiluk, Yip, DeVito, Carroll and Sofuoglu2019). Blunted reactivity to reward-predicting stimuli and receipt of non-drug rewards has been observed in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum among patients receiving medication for OUD (i.e. methadone) (Moningka et al., Reference Moningka, Lichenstein, Worhunsky, DeVito, Scheinost and Yip2019). This finding suggests that anhedonia and reward processing remain untreated by one of the most effective first-line medications for OUD. Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) often includes behavioral activation to increase engagement in rewarding life activities. However, being physically present with a natural reinforcer may not counter cognitive biases that divert attention away from natural reward. In that regard, a recent review found no evidence that behavioral activation (or CBT as a whole) significantly improves anhedonia in chronic opioid users (Kiluk et al., Reference Kiluk, Yip, DeVito, Carroll and Sofuoglu2019). Specific attentional training in savoring may be needed to overcome attentional bias and increase sensory-perceptual contact with the rewarding stimulus (Garland, Reference Garland2020).

Savoring involves attending to the pleasant features (e.g. visual, auditory, gustatory, olfactory, tactile, or kinesthetic) of a naturally rewarding stimulus and the resultant positive emotions and pleasurable sensations that emerge during contact with the stimulus (Bryant & Veroff, Reference Bryant and Veroff2017). Mindfulness training is theorized to promote savoring of natural rewards by stabilizing and reorienting attention from distraction onto the pleasant stimulus, and then by deepening meta-awareness of positive emotional and interoceptive responses to the stimulus (Garland, Reference Garland2020). In that regard, a novel cognitive intervention, Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) unites training in mindfulness and savoring skills to remediate reward dysregulation among chronic opioid users. MORE has been tested in multiple randomized controlled trials (RCTs) for chronic pain patients prescribed LTOT. Across these trials, MORE decreased chronic pain symptoms, opioid misuse, craving, and illicit drug use, while simultaneously increasing positive affect and self-reported savoring (Cooperman, Hanley, Kline, & Garland, Reference Cooperman, Hanley, Kline and Garland2021a; Garland et al., Reference Garland, Manusov, Froeliger, Kelly, Williams and Howard2014b, Reference Garland, Hanley, Riquino, Reese, Baker, Salas and Howard2019c, Reference Garland, Hanley, Nakamura, Baker, Reese, Riquino and Howard2021). Additionally, pilot studies found MORE increased cardiac-autonomic and ERP markers of attention to natural reward cues (Garland, Froeliger, & Howard, Reference Garland, Froeliger and Howard2014a, Reference Garland, Froeliger and Howard2015b, Reference Garland, Atchley, Hanley, Zubieta and Froeliger2019a). However, no study has assessed whether MORE's effects on modulating neurophysiological responses to natural reward cues are associated with improvements in anhedonia among people on LTOT.

To that end, we conducted an ancillary mechanistic sub-study overlaid on a clinical trial (NCT02935621) to test whether participation in MORE v. an active control condition [supportive group (SG) psychotherapy] would occasion increased LPP and SCL responses during attention to natural reward cues, and whether such increased neurophysiological responses would be associated with clinical improvements in anhedonia. We hypothesized that (1) MORE would be associated with significantly greater increases in ‘bottom-up’ neurophysiological responses to natural reward cues than the SG, (2) MORE would increase the capacity to up-regulate these neurophysiological responses during savoring, and (3) the effects of MORE on reducing subjective anhedonia would be associated with increases in neurophysiological response to natural reward cues.

Materials and methods

Participants

In this ancillary mechanistic study, we added a neurophysiological assessment protocol to an ongoing clinical trial (NCT02935621) where EEG outcomes were not proposed as part of the original clinical trial design. This mechanistic study evaluated neurophysiological (EEG and SCL) data from a sample of Veterans with chronic pain receiving LTOT (N = 63). Primary clinical outcomes from NCT02935621 will be reported elsewhere. Individuals who provided complete pre-post treatment EEG data (Table 1) were included in the present analysis (see online Supplementary material for CONSORT Diagram). Participants (83% male; mean age = 60.3 ± 10.1 years) were recruited from VA primary care and pain clinics, and met inclusion criteria if they had taken opioids for at least the past 90 days (mean opioid use duration = 9.8 ± 8.3 years) and reported chronic non-cancer pain (mean pain duration = 15.9 ± 13.9 years). The mean (±s.d.) opioid dose in morphine milligram equivalents was 71.7 ± 168.1 mg. On the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller and Dunbar1998), 63% met the criteria for major depressive disorder (MDD), 16% for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), and 11% for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Participants were excluded if they had previously engaged in a formal MBI or for active suicidality or psychosis as assessed by the MINI. Participants were financially compensated. The protocol was approved by the University of Utah IRB and VA Salt Lake City Health Care System Research and Development Committee, and all procedures complied with standards set forth in the Helsinki Declaration of 1975.

Table 1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics (N = 63) of patients with chronic pain on long-term opioid therapy (LTOT) treated with Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) or a supportive group (SG) psychotherapy control condition

a Subjects were allowed to enter more than one opioid type. SHAPS, Snaith Hamilton Anhedonia and Pleasure Scale.

Procedures

Following screening, participants who gave written informed consent completed demographic and clinical assessments, including the Snaith Hamilton Anhedonia and Pleasure Scale (SHAPS) (Snaith et al., Reference Snaith, Hamilton, Morley, Humayan, Hargreaves and Trigwell1995). Then, they completed a laboratory-based, natural reward responsiveness task (see below) during which EEG and SCL were recorded. Participants were informed that they would be randomized to a behavioral treatment group that would help them to cope with pain, stress, and opioid-related problems by providing either mindfulness training or group support. After the pre-treatment assessment, participants were randomly allocated to MORE or an SG control using simple randomization in blocks of varying sizes (2–4) to preserve allocation unpredictability. After the 8-week MORE and SG treatments, participants returned to the laboratory to again complete the natural reward responsiveness task. The SHAPS was assessed again at post-treatment and 2- and 4-month follow-ups.

Interventions

The manualized MORE intervention program provided training in mindfulness, reappraisal, and savoring skills as techniques to cope with opioid craving, pain, and negative affect. Group sessions were 2 h long and led by a psychologist. Sessions included training in mindful breathing and body scan techniques to help patients self-regulate pain and opioid craving, reappraisal training to decrease negative emotions, and savoring training to amplify natural reward processing. With respect to savoring, specifically, participants were trained to mindfully focus attention on the sensory features of a pleasant object presented in the session (e.g. a rose), and then to appreciate and enjoy any positive emotions or pleasurable body sensations arising during the encounter with the pleasant stimulus. Then, participants were asked to engage in daily 15 min home practice of mindfulness as well as practicing savoring with naturally-occurring pleasant objects and events in everyday life.

To control for non-specific factors including attention by a caring professional, therapeutic expectancy, and social support, we employed a manualized active SG control in this study. The SG consisted of 8 weekly, 2 h, process-oriented, Rogerian group psychotherapy sessions, in which a psychologist facilitated emotional expression and discussion of topics pertinent to chronic pain and opioid use/misuse. This client-centered SG format was validated in prior RCTs of MORE (Garland et al., Reference Garland, Manusov, Froeliger, Kelly, Williams and Howard2014b, Reference Garland, Hanley, Riquino, Reese, Baker, Salas and Howard2019c). SG participants were asked to engage at home in 15 min of journaling a day on chronic pain and opioid-related themes. To prevent treatment diffusion, participants in the SG condition were instructed to not engage in mindfulness training during the course of the study. A clinician with 15+ years of experience conducted clinical supervision and reviewed session recordings to monitor therapist adherence to the MORE and SG treatment manuals and maintain intervention fidelity.

Assessment of natural reward responsiveness

In an experimental laboratory session, participants were presented with images representing natural rewards or neutral cues. On each trial, participants were first shown a fixation cross for 500 ms, followed by 250–500 ms jittered blank screen and then an image and instruction label for 5000 ms. Across 64 trials, cues were presented in a randomized, event-related design.

Participants were instructed to View or Regulate responses to natural reward stimuli. On View trials, participants were instructed to simply attend to images of naturally rewarding stimuli (e.g. social affiliation, natural beauty, athletic victories, etc.) validated in prior studies (Garland, Bryan, Nakamura, Froeliger, & Howard, Reference Garland, Bryan, Nakamura, Froeliger and Howard2017c) or neutral images (e.g. people with neutral facial expressions, household objects) whose basic visual properties were matched to the natural reward cues. On Regulate trials, to approximate mindful savoring techniques and conform with typical ‘increase positive’ instructions on emotion regulation tasks (Froeliger et al., Reference Froeliger, Mathew, McConnell, Eichberg, Saladin, Carpenter and Garland2017), participants were instructed to imagine experiencing the positive event occurring in the image, and to focus on and appreciate the pleasant aspects of the image and their own positive emotional response to the image. In a training session prior to psychophysiological assessment, participants practiced this regulatory strategy and described their experience to a trained research assistant to ensure comprehension of the instructions. Psychophysiological assessment did not commence until participants could accurately describe the implementation of regulatory instruction.

EEG

EEG was continuously recorded from 10 midline scalp sites (Fz, F3, F4, FC1, FC2, FCz, Cz, CP1, CP2, PZ) using an active sensor cap with Ag/AgCl electrodes (actiCap GmbH, Herrsching, Germany). All recordings were collected by an actiCHamp amplifier (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Data were acquired at a sampling rate of 500 Hz, a resolution of 0.489 μV and an amplification cutoff of 140 Hz, with impedances kept below 10 kΩ.

EEG data preprocessing was conducted using a custom MATLAB (MATLAB version 9.3.0,.713579 (R2017b), 2017) script set developed by the authors, containing both original and EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, Reference Delorme and Makeig2004) functions, while ERP analysis was performed in the Psychophysiological Toolbox (Bernat, Williams, & Gehring, Reference Bernat, Williams and Gehring2005). A low-pass filter of 50 Hz was applied to the continuous data, and then ERP epochs were created beginning 1000 ms pre-stimulus and ending 2000 ms post-stimulus. For each individual, epochs were ranked according to the number of extreme (> ± 150 mV) data points across all channels, and the worst 5% of epochs were removed. Additionally, individual channels were interpolated across all data if they exceeded the threshold of 5 standard deviations in the domains of kurtosis and activity probability. After baseline (500–100 ms pre-stimulus) correction occurred, each epoch was evaluated separately and channels with extreme (> ± 150 mV) data points were interpolated only for that epoch, while epochs with more than two bad channels were rejected and removed from the data. A final visual inspection was conducted to remove epochs with unusual artifacts. ERP component scores were extracted and exported for further statistical analyses. For hypothesis testing, we assessed activity at Pz where the LPP was maximal, consistent with previous research (Foti & Hajcak, Reference Foti and Hajcak2008; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000).

Skin conductance

Skin conductance sensors were placed on the middle phalanx of the index and middle fingers of the non-dominant hand. A BIOPAC MP160 amplifier provided a constant voltage (0.5 V) across the two electrodes. Change relative to a 500 ms pre-stimulus baseline was computed in half-second epochs.

Anhedonia

The SHAPS (Snaith et al., Reference Snaith, Hamilton, Morley, Humayan, Hargreaves and Trigwell1995) consists of 14 items tapping the pleasure experienced from a variety of natural rewards (e.g. being with family, a beautiful landscape, receiving praise), rated on a Likert-type scale (1 = strongly agree, 4 = strongly disagree). Although both 2- and 4-point SHAPS scoring formats have been reported (Trøstheim et al., Reference Trøstheim, Eikemo, Meir, Hansen, Paul, Kroll and Leknes2020), because we and others have found the greater variability of the 4-point scoring format to be sensitive to effects of chronic pain and substance use (e.g. opioids) (Trøstheim et al., Reference Trøstheim, Eikemo, Meir, Hansen, Paul, Kroll and Leknes2020), we selected the 4-point scoring format for this study. SHAPS total scores ranged from 14 to 56, with higher scores indicating higher levels of anhedonia.

Positive affect

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule-short form (PANAS-SF) was used to assess positive affect with 10 items rated on a Likert-type scale (1 = not at all, 5 = extremely) (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988).

Reward experience

One item from the Applied Mindfulness Process Scale (AMPS; Li, Black, & Garland, Reference Li, Black and Garland2016), ‘In the last week, I enjoyed the little things in life more fully’, was used as a process measure at pre- and post-treatment to assess the impact of MORE skills (v. the SG) on reward experience, rated on a Likert-type scale (0 = not at all, 4 = extremely).

Statistical analysis

This mechanistic study was powered to detect the effects of MORE on neurophysiological processes. Power analysis conducted with G*power 3.1 indicated that 60 subjects were required to detect a statistically significant (p < 0.05, two-tailed), small-moderate effect size (η partial2 > 0.04) ‘within-between’ interaction via repeated-measures ANOVA; power = 0.80.

Given the canonical time window of the LPP and our a priori hypothesis, we used repeated-measures ANOVA (RM-ANOVA) to examine the effects of treatment on EEG response from 400 to 2000 ms to capture the maximal peak deflection of the LPP. Following convention (Hajcak & Foti, Reference Hajcak and Foti2020; Schupp et al., Reference Schupp, Cuthbert, Bradley, Cacioppo, Ito and Lang2000), we defined the LPP as average voltage in successive time windows: 300–600, 600–1000, 1000–1500, and 1500–2000 ms. In contrast, we examined the effects of treatment on SCL across the entire stimulus viewing period from 0 to 5000 ms in 500 ms windows, because we did not have a strong a priori hypothesis about the time window whereby MORE would exert its effect on sympathetic responses. Prior to hypothesis testing, we assessed for baseline equivalence in the pre-treatment data, and then adjusted the LPP and SCL for neutral stimulus processing by subtracting activation to neutral cues from activation to reward cues for each epoch. To test our first hypothesis that MORE would be associated with significantly greater increases in ‘bottom-up’ reward responsiveness than the SG over time, we assessed the interaction of Treatment (MORE v. SG) with Time (Pre- v. Post-treatment) on the physiological response to natural reward stimuli. To test our second hypothesis that MORE may amplify ‘top-down’ conscious up-regulation of reward responsiveness through savoring, we assessed the interaction of Treatment(MORE v. SG) with Strategy (View v. Regulate) and Time (Pre- v. Post-treatment) on the physiological response to natural reward stimuli. Both LPP and SCL analyses tested the main effect of time window, as well as its interaction with Treatment, Time, and Strategy, to determine if effects differed across the time-course of the physiological signal. Because MORE and SG differed at baseline in GAD baseline diagnosis, and because opioid dose could confound physiological responses, we controlled for these variables in sensitivity analyses. Significant (p < 0.05) main effects and interactions from RM-ANOVAs were investigated with Bonferroni-adjusted planned post hoc contrasts, with violations of sphericity addressed with Greenhouse Geiser-corrected df.

Next, to perform an intention-to-treat analysis of the effects of treatment on SHAPS scores over time, we used linear mixed modeling (LMM) in SPSS 27 with maximum likelihood estimation and fixed effects consisting of a time factor and between-subjects treatment factor (MORE v. SG). The parameter of interest was the Treatment × Time interaction, and the model was specified with a patient-level random intercept and a repeated covariance structure determined by AICC information criteria. One-sample t tests were used to compare anhedonia scores to meta-analytically derived reference values for healthy subjects. In exploratory post-hoc analyses, we used similar LMMs to examine treatment effects on PANAS and AMPS item scores.

Finally, we examined correlations between neurophysiological responses (LPP and SCL) and anhedonia scores in the follow-up period. In an exploratory post-hoc analysis, we also examined neurophysiological correlates of positive affect and reward experience. We then conducted a path analysis in the R version 4.0.3 Lavaan package using maximum likelihood estimation with robust (Huber–White) standard errors to evaluate neurophysiological responses as a mediator of treatment effects (MORE v. SG) on anhedonia.

Results

Baseline equivalence

There was no significant between-groups difference by Treatment condition on the LPP (F 1,61 = 2.45, p = 0.12, η partial2 = 0.03) or SCL at baseline (F 1,51 = 2.24, p = 0.14, η partial2 = 0.04), nor were the Treatment × Strategy or Treatment × Strategy × Window interactions significant, demonstrating baseline equivalence between groups.

Treatment effects on LPP response

Effects of treatment on the LPP (Fig. 1) were examined with RM-ANOVA. A significant Treatment × Time interaction was observed, F 1,61 = 4.80, p = 0.03, η partial2 = 0.07, indicating that regardless of strategy, MORE was associated with significantly greater increases in LPP response to natural reward cues across the LPP than the SG, regardless of strategy (Fig. 2). However, neither the Treatment × Strategy × Time interaction (F 1,61 = 0.006, p = 0.94, η partial2 < 0.001), nor the Treatment × Strategy × Time × Window interaction (F 3,183 = 1.84, p = 0.16, η partial2 = 0.03), was significant, indicating that the effect of MORE (relative to the SG) did not significantly differ between regulatory strategies over time; relative to the SG, MORE increased LPP from pre- to post-treatment to a comparable degree for both the View and Regulate conditions across the entire LPP and did not differ by time window. Similarly, neither the main effect of Time, the main effect of Strategy, nor the main effect of Treatment was significant. In the sensitivity analysis, the Treatment × Time interaction remained significant, F 1,58 = 5.41, p = 0.02, η partial2 = 0.09.

Fig. 1. Changes in parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) during (a) (top left) passive viewing of natural reward (i.e. View trials) from pre- to post-treatment with Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE); (b) (top right) up-regulation of responding to natural reward (i.e. Regulate trials) from pre- to post-treatment with MORE; (c) (bottom left) passive viewing of natural reward (i.e. View trials) from pre- to post-treatment with supportive group (SG) psychotherapy; (d) (bottom right) up-regulation of responding to natural reward (i.e. Regulate trials) from pre- to post-treatment with SG.

Fig. 2. Difference waves (post-treatment minus pre-treatment) depicting effects of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) response to natural reward cues, averaged across View and Regulate trials.

Treatment effects on SCL response

Effects of treatment on SCL (Fig. 3) were examined with RM-ANOVA. Emerging from this analysis was a significant Treatment × Time × Window interaction, F 1.5,91.4 = 4.54, p = 0.020, η partial2 = 0.07. Inspection of the data indicated that regardless of strategy, MORE was associated with significantly greater increases in SCL response to natural reward cues than the SG; simple contrasts indicated this treatment effect reached statistical significance around the 2000 ms time window, and peaked by 4500 ms (F 1,58 = 7.49, p = 0.008, η partial2 = 0.11). However, none of the other higher-order interaction effects were significant, and neither the main effect of Time, the main effect of Strategy, nor the main effect of Treatment was significant. In the sensitivity analysis, the Treatment × Time × Window interaction remained significant, F 1.5,495 = 4.28, p = 0.025, η partial2 = 0.07.

Fig. 3. Difference waves (post-treatment minus pre-treatment) depicting effects of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on SCL response to natural reward cues, averaged across View and Regulate trials for 500 ms windows through the 5 s cue presentation.

Treatment effects on anhedonia, positive affect, and reward experience

In LMM of SHAPS scores, the Treatment × Time interaction was significant, F (1,47.09) = 6.42, p = 0.015, indicating that participants in MORE exhibited a greater decrease (Δ SHAPS = −3.00 ± 1.14) in anhedonia through the 4-month follow-up than those in the SG (Δ SHAPS = 0.52 ± 1.16). At the pre-randomization assessment, SHAPS scores among participants (23.6 ± 0.8) significantly differed (p < 0.001) from the established SHAPS reference value for healthy individuals (20.2 ± 0.03). By 4-month follow-up, SHAPS scores among participants in MORE (21.3 ± 1.3) did not significantly differ (p = 0.41) from the established SHAPS healthy reference value, whereas SHAPS scores among SG participants (22.9 ± 1.1) remained significantly different from the SHAPS healthy reference value (p = 0.018).

With regard to PANAS positive affect, although the main effect of Time was significant F (1,51.66) = 7.41, p < 0.001, the Treatment × Time interaction was not, F (1,51.66) = 1.43, p = 0.25, indicating that participants in both MORE and those in supportive psychotherapy reported significant increases in positive affect over time. Finally, with regard to the AMPS reward experience item, the Treatment × Time interaction was significant, F (1,57.57) = 4.13, p = 0.047, indicating that participants exhibited a greater increase in reward experience by post-treatment than the SG.

Association between neurophysiological mechanisms and anhedonia, positive affect, and reward experience

Across the entire sample, pre-post increases in LPP during the regulate strategy correlated with lower SHAPS scores throughout the follow-up, r = −0.34, p = 0.01. Controlling for opioid use and MDD, GAD, and PTSD diagnoses as potential confounders, the correlation between increases in the LPP during the regulate strategy and decreased anhedonia remained significant (ps ranged from 0.021 to 0.029). Given this association, we conducted a path analysis with the R Lavaan package (Fig. 4). The indirect effect (a × b) was significant (p = 0.012, 95% CI 0.30–2.35), demonstrating that increased LPP during the regulate strategy mediated the effect of MORE on reducing anhedonia. However, neither SCL nor the LPP during the view strategy was significantly associated with anhedonia. Although positive affect was not associated with SCL or LPP, the AMPS reward experience item at post-treatment was positively correlated with the LPP (1500–2000 ms window) during viewing of natural reward cues, r = 0.26, p = 0.047.

Fig. 4. Path model indicating that the effect of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on reducing anhedonia by 4-month follow-up was statistically mediated by increasing parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) to natural reward cues.

Discussion

Here we obtained neurophysiological evidence in support of the hypothesis that MORE increases motivated attention to natural reward cues in chronic pain patients on LTOT – effects that were associated with decreased anhedonia. Relative to an active SG psychotherapy control condition, participants in the MORE group exhibited heightened LPP and SCL responses to natural reward stimuli. In individual difference analyses, increases in LPP activity during savoring predicted reductions in anhedonia symptoms through a 4-month follow-up.

Findings suggest that training in attention allocation to the positive exteroceptive and interoceptive features of a stimulus context (i.e. savoring) through MORE can amplify LPP markers of motivated attention to natural reward stimuli. MORE's effects were similar on View and Regulate trials; though MORE provides training in proactive up-regulation of reward via savoring, it seems to enhance motivated attention to natural reward cues in a bottom-up fashion even when no conscious attempts to savor are made. Hypothetically, repeated practice of savoring may restructure the reward value of non-drug rewards, such that over time, no proactive efforts to up-regulate reward are needed; indeed, automatic (implicit) emotion regulation has been observed and indexed by the LPP (Zhang & Zhou, Reference Zhang and Zhou2014) – with top-down regulation of responding to emotional stimuli exerting long-lasting, dose-dependent effects on bottom-up neural reactivity to those stimuli (Denny, Inhoff, Zerubavel, Davachi, & Ochsner, Reference Denny, Inhoff, Zerubavel, Davachi and Ochsner2015). These data replicate and extend prior findings of enhanced LPP responses to natural reward cues from smaller pilot studies of MORE (Garland, Froeliger, & Howard, Reference Garland, Froeliger and Howard2015b, Reference Garland, Atchley, Hanley, Zubieta and Froeliger2019a). Hypothetically, the increases in LPP response during attention to natural reward cues observed in the present study might be undergirded by corticostriatal activation. This speculation is consistent with neuroimaging data indicating that up-regulation of positive emotion is subserved by activation in medial PFC, caudate, and putamen (Li et al., Reference Li, Yin, Feng, Hu, Ding and Chen2018), and pilot work in smokers indicating that MORE enhances activity in these same brain regions during savoring (Froeliger et al., Reference Froeliger, Mathew, McConnell, Eichberg, Saladin, Carpenter and Garland2017).

SCL during attention to natural reward cues also increased following 8 weeks of treatment with MORE, with no difference between the View and Regulate conditions. In contrast, SCL and the LPP decreased among participants in the SG – likely a function of habituation. Skin conductance habituation has been reliably demonstrated during viewing of positive affective images (Codispoti, Ferrari, & Bradley, Reference Codispoti, Ferrari and Bradley2006). Rather than exhibiting the normative habituation response, participants in MORE exhibited heightened SCL to natural reward stimuli over time. SCL is a known index of the attentional orienting response and sympathetic arousal during threat and reward (Löw, Lang, Smith, & Bradley, Reference Löw, Lang, Smith and Bradley2008). The observed LPP and SCL effects suggest that MORE increased attention to natural reward stimuli and enhanced subsequent motivational arousal.

Further, based on a recent systematic review (Kiluk et al., Reference Kiluk, Yip, DeVito, Carroll and Sofuoglu2019), these data provide the first evidence that a behavioral intervention of any kind can reduce anhedonia in people with chronic pain on LTOT. MORE significantly decreased anhedonia symptoms, effects that were associated with increased LPP during attention to natural reward cues during savoring, suggesting that this technique – integral to MORE – may be especially important for remediating reward processing deficits. In that regard, MORE also increased self-reported reward experience as measured by a mindfulness process measure. However, despite prior studies where MORE has shown efficacy for increasing positive affect (Garland et al., Reference Garland, Bryan, Finan, Thomas, Priddy, Riquino and Howard2017b, Reference Garland, Hanley, Riquino, Reese, Baker, Salas and Howard2019c), in the present study both MORE and supportive psychotherapy increased positive affect to a comparable degree. These findings suggest that MORE has unique effects on targeting anhedonia above and beyond the generalized improvements in emotional well-being produced by supportive psychotherapy. That said, increased SCL occasioned by MORE was not correlated with improved anhedonia, so converging sympathetic evidence was not obtained. In addition, multiple other factors could have confounded the observed association between increased LPP during savoring and decreased anhedonia, including changes in anxiety, depression, PTSD – psychological variables that have all been shown to be treated by MORE in prior studies (Cooperman et al., Reference Cooperman, Hanley, Kline and Garland2021a; Garland, Roberts-Lewis, Tronnier, Graves, & Kelley, Reference Garland, Roberts-Lewis, Tronnier, Graves and Kelley2016). Replication studies should examine associations between the neurophysiological effects of MORE and improvement of such factors to determine whether increased LPP during savoring is more robustly and specifically related to anhedonia than to other measures.

The current study had several other important limitations. First, because that the present analysis did not examine MORE's effects during attention to negative emotional stimuli, it is possible that MORE might increase attentional responses to emotionally salient stimuli of any valence. Yet, prior studies have indicated that MORE decreases attention to both negatively valenced stimuli (Garland et al., Reference Garland, Bryan, Priddy, Riquino, Froeliger and Howard2019b; Garland & Howard, Reference Garland and Howard2013) and drug-related stimuli (Garland, Baker, & Howard, Reference Garland, Baker and Howard2017a, Reference Garland, Atchley, Hanley, Zubieta and Froeliger2019a), suggesting that the observed increases in neurophysiological response might be selective for positive stimuli (i.e. natural reward cues). The selectivity of this effect should be determined in future studies involving a range of stimulus valences. Furthermore, without a corresponding behavioral measure of reward such as self-reported liking or wanting, or a performance-based measure of motivation, like the EEFfrT (Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert, & Zald, Reference Treadway, Buckholtz, Schwartzman, Lambert and Zald2009), the task used in the present study cannot assess whether MORE impacts reward responsiveness above and beyond attention to natural reward cues. Next, a high-density electrode montage would have allowed source localization for inferences about the brain networks involved. However, given the vulnerable nature of the study participants, we selected a limited set of electrodes to minimize participant burden. Also, because participants were instructed to take opioids as prescribed on the day of the experiment to prevent withdrawal-related cognitive and neurophysiological disturbances, the acute pharmacological effects of opioids may have influenced neurophysiological responses. That said, the observed effects of MORE on LPP responses remained significant in sensitivity analyses controlling for opioid dose. Future studies could examine mindfulness-induced modulation of EEG oscillations in opioid-naïve chronic pain patients and in patients following stabilized medical titration from LTOT. Finally, the study had a modest sample size comprised largely of older white male veterans, limiting the generalizability of study findings; future investigations should employ larger, more racially diverse samples to examine associations between neurophysiological responses during attention to natural rewards and clinical outcomes.

In summary, following 8 weeks of treatment with MORE, patients receiving LTOT for chronic pain exhibited increased electrocortical and sympathetic nervous system responses during attention to natural reward cues that predicted decreases in anhedonia 4 months after treatment ended. In light of neurophysiological evidence of clinical target engagement, adequately powered, full-scale clinical trials are now needed to test the efficacy of MORE as a means of treating anhedonia and reward deficits among chronic opioid users, people with chronic pain, and those at risk for OUD. Given that anhedonia is the hallmark of depression, and depression may exacerbate the risk for opioid misuse and OUD among chronic pain patients (Emery & Akil, Reference Emery and Akil2020), future, large-scale trials should aim to replicate these effects in patients on LTOT with chronic pain and comorbid MDD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721003834

Author contributions

Eric L. Garland: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, funding acquisition, project administration, investigation, resources, data curation, writing – original and editing. Spencer Fix: software, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing-original and editing, visualization, investigation. Justin Hudak: software, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing-editing, visualization, investigation. Edward Bernat: software, methodology, formal analysis, data curation, writing-editing, visualization. Yoshio Nakamura: conceptualization, methodology, funding acquisition, interpretation, writing – review and editing. Adam W. Hanley: conceptualization, interpretation, writing – review and editing. Gary Donaldson: conceptualization, methodology, funding acquisition, writing – editing. Brett Froeliger: software, methodology, conceptualization, writing – editing. William R. Marchand: conceptualization, resources, interpretation, funding acquisition, writing – review and editing, supervision, conceptualization.

Financial support

This work was supported by W81XWH-16-1-0522 from the Department of Defense (PI: Garland), R01DA042033 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (PI: Garland), and R61AT009296 from the National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (PI: Garland). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Department of Defense or National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of interest

Eric Garland, Ph.D., LCSW is the Director of the Center on Mindfulness and Integrative Health Intervention Development. The Center provides Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE), mindfulness-based therapy, and cognitive behavioral therapy in the context of research trials for no cost to research participants; however, Dr Garland has received honoraria and payment for delivering seminars, lectures, and teaching engagements (related to training clinicians in mindfulness) sponsored by institutions of higher education, government agencies, academic teaching hospitals, and medical centers. Dr Garland also receives royalties from the sale of books related to MORE, and is a consultant of and licensor to BehaVR, LLC. The remaining authors have nothing to disclose.

References

Armstrong, T., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 704723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernat, E. M., Williams, W. J., & Gehring, W. J. (2005). Decomposing ERP time–frequency energy using PCA. Elsevier Clinical Neurophysiology, 116, 13141334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 1, 276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brailean, A. M., Koster, E. H. W., Hoorelbeke, K., & De Raedt, R. (2014). Attentional modulation by reward and punishment cues in relation to depressive symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 45, 351359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, F. B., & Veroff, J. (2017) Savoring: A new model of positive experience. New Jersey: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, S. F., Budman, S. H., Fernandez, K. C., Houle, B., Benoit, C., Katz, N., & Jamison, R. N. (2007). Development and validation of the current opioid misuse measure. Pain, 130, 144156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chou, R., Fanciullo, G. J., Fine, P. G., Adler, J. A., Ballantyne, J. C., Davies, P., … American Pain Society-American Academy of Pain Medicine Opioids Guidelines Panel. (2009) Clinical guidelines for the use of chronic opioid therapy in chronic noncancer pain. The Journal of Pain, 10, 113130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chou, R., Turner, J. A., Devine, E. B., Hansen, R. N., Sullivan, S. D., Blazina, I., … Deyo, R. A. (2015). The effectiveness and risks of long-term opioid therapy for chronic pain: A systematic review for a national institutes of health pathways to prevention workshop. Annals of Internal Medicine, 162, 276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2006). Repetitive picture processing: Autonomic and cortical correlates. Brain Research, 1068, 213220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooperman, N., Hanley, A. W., Kline, A., & Garland, E. L. (2021a). A pilot randomized clinical trial of mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement as an adjunct to methadone treatment for people with opioid use disorder and chronic pain: Impact on illicit drug use, health, and well-being. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Psychology, 52, 95111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado, M. R., Gillis, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Regulating the expectation of reward via cognitive strategies. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 880881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134, 921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denny, B. T., Inhoff, M. C., Zerubavel, N., Davachi, L., & Ochsner, K. N. (2015). Getting over it: Long-lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response. Sage Publications Sage CA: Los Angeles. CA Psychological Science, 26, 13771388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emery, M. A., & Akil, H. (2020). Endogenous opioids at the intersection of opioid addiction, pain, and depression: The search for a precision medicine approach. Annual Reviews Annual review of neuroscience, 43, 355374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foti, D., & Hajcak, G. (2008). Deconstructing reappraisal: Descriptions preceding arousing pictures modulate the subsequent neural response. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 977988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Froeliger, B., Mathew, A. R., McConnell, P. A., Eichberg, C., Saladin, M. E., Carpenter, M. J., & Garland, E. L. (2017). Restructuring reward mechanisms in nicotine addiction: A pilot fMRI study of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement for cigarette smokers. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 2017, e7018014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L. (2020). Psychosocial intervention and the reward system in pain and opioid misuse: New opportunities and directions. LWW Pain, 161, 26592666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Atchley, R. M., Hanley, A. W., Zubieta, J.-K., & Froeliger, B. (2019a). Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement remediates hedonic dysregulation in opioid users: Neural and affective evidence of target engagement. Science Advances, 5, eaax1569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Baker, A. K., & Howard, M. O. (2017a). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement reduces opioid attentional bias among prescription opioid-treated chronic pain patients. Journal of the Society for Social Work and Research, 8, 493509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garland, E. L., Bryan, C. J., Finan, P. H., Thomas, E. A., Priddy, S. E., Riquino, M. R., & Howard, M. O. (2017b). Pain, hedonic regulation, and opioid misuse: Modulation of momentary experience by Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement in opioid-treated chronic pain patients. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 173, S65S72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Bryan, C. J., Nakamura, Y., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (2017c). Deficits in autonomic indices of emotion regulation and reward processing associated with prescription opioid use and misuse. Psychopharmacology, 234, 621629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Bryan, M. A., Priddy, S. E., Riquino, M. R., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (2019b). Effects of mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement versus social support on negative affective interference during inhibitory control among opioid-treated chronic pain patients: A pilot mechanistic study. Oxford University Press US Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 53, 865876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (2014a). Effects of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement on reward responsiveness and opioid cue-reactivity. Psychopharmacology, 231, 32293238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (2015a). Allostatic dysregulation of natural reward processing in prescription opioid misuse: Autonomic and attentional evidence. Biological Psychology, 105, 124129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Froeliger, B., & Howard, M. O. (2015b). Neurophysiological evidence for remediation of reward processing deficits in chronic pain and opioid misuse following treatment with Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement: Exploratory ERP findings from a pilot RCT. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 38, 327336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Froeliger, B., Zeidan, F., Partin, K., & Howard, M. O. (2013). The downward spiral of chronic pain, prescription opioid misuse, and addiction: Cognitive, affective, and neuropsychopharmacologic pathways. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 25972607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Hanley, A. W., Nakamura, Y., Baker, A. K., Reese, S. E., Riquino, M. R., … Howard, M.O. (2021) Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement for opioid misuse and chronic pain in primary care: A randomized clinical trial. Under review.Google Scholar
Garland, E. L., Hanley, A. W., Riquino, M. R., Reese, S. E., Baker, A. K., Salas, K., … Howard, M. O. (2019c). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement reduces opioid misuse risk via analgesic and positive psychological mechanisms: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 87, 927940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., & Howard, M. O. (2013). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement reduces pain attentional bias in chronic pain patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 82, 311318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Manusov, E. G., Froeliger, B., Kelly, A., Williams, J. M., & Howard, M. O. (2014b). Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse: Results from an early-stage randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Roberts-Lewis, A., Tronnier, C. D., Graves, R., & Kelley, K. (2016). Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement versus CBT for co-occurring substance dependence, traumatic stress, and psychiatric disorders: Proximal outcomes from a pragmatic randomized trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 77, 716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E. L., Trøstheim, M., Eikemo, M., Ernst, G., & Leknes, S. (2020). Anhedonia in chronic pain and prescription opioid misuse. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19771988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajcak, G., & Foti, D. (2020). Significance?…Significance! Empirical, methodological, and theoretical connections between the late positive potential and P300 as neural responses to stimulus significance: An integrative review. Psychophysiology, 57, e13570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudak, J., Prince, K. C., Marchand, W. R., Nakamura, Y., Hanley, A. W., Bryan, C. J., … Garland, E. L. (2021). The temporal dynamics of emotion dysregulation in prescription opioid misuse. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 104, 110024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huhn, A. S., Brooner, R. K., Sweeney, M. M., Antoine, D., Hammond, A. S., Ayaz, H., & Dunn, K. E. (2021). The association of prefrontal cortex response during a natural reward cue-reactivity paradigm, anhedonia, and demoralization in persons maintained on methadone. Addictive Behaviors, 113, 106673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S. W., & North, R. A. (1992). Opioids excite dopamine neurons by hyperpolarization of local interneurons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 483488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiluk, B. D., Yip, S. W., DeVito, E. E., Carroll, K. M., & Sofuoglu, M. (2019). Anhedonia as a key clinical feature in the maintenance and treatment of opioid use disorder. SAGE Publications Inc Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 11901206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F. (2020). Neurobiology of opioid addiction: Opponent process, hyperkatifeia, and negative reinforcement. Biological Psychiatry, 87, 4453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, F., Yin, S., Feng, P., Hu, N., Ding, C., & Chen, A. (2018). The cognitive up- and down-regulation of positive emotion: Evidence from behavior, electrophysiology, and neuroimaging. Biological Psychology, 136, 5766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, M. J., Black, D. S., & Garland, E. L. (2016). The Applied Mindfulness Process Scale (AMPS): A process measure for evaluating mindfulness-based interventions. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Löw, A., Lang, P. J., Smith, J. C., & Bradley, M. M. (2008). Both predator and prey: Emotional arousal in threat and reward. SAGE Publications Inc Psychological Science, 19, 865873.Google ScholarPubMed
Lubman, D. I., Yucel, M., Kettle, J. W., Scaffidi, A., Mackenzie, T., Simmons, J. G., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Responsiveness to drug cues and natural rewards in opiate addiction: Associations with later heroin use. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 205212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MATLAB version 9.3.0.713579 (R2017b) (2017). Natick. Massachusetts: The Mathworks, Inc.Google Scholar
Moningka, H., Lichenstein, S., Worhunsky, P. D., DeVito, E. E., Scheinost, D., & Yip, S. W. (2019). Can neuroimaging help combat the opioid epidemic? A systematic review of clinical and pharmacological challenge fMRI studies with recommendations for future research. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 259273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oe, M., Schumacher, S., Schnyder, U., Mueller-Pfeiffer, C., Wilhelm, F. H., Kuelen, E., & Martin-Soelch, C. (2016). Discriminative evaluative conditioning in the long-term after severe accidental injury. Psychiatry Research, 240, 144150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rømer Thomsen, K. (2015). Measuring anhedonia: Impaired ability to pursue, experience, and learn about reward. Frontiers in Psychology, 0, 123.Google Scholar
Rømer Thomsen, K., Whybrow, P. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Reconceptualizing anhedonia: Novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 49.Google ScholarPubMed
Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37, 257261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., … Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 2233, quiz 34–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, D. V., Rigney, A. E., & Delgado, M. R. (2016). Distinct reward properties are encoded via corticostriatal interactions. Scientific Reports, 6, 20093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A., Hargreaves, D., & Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale. Cambridge University Press The British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 99103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: The orienting reflex. Annual Reviews 4139 El Camino Way, PO Box 10139, Palo Alto, CA 94303-0139, USA. Annual Review of Physiology, 25, 545580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spagnolo, P. A., Kimes, A., Schwandt, M. L., Shokri-Kojori, E., Thada, S., Phillips, K. A., … Heilig, M. (2019). Striatal dopamine release in response to morphine: A [11C]raclopride positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Biological Psychiatry, 86, 356364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treadway, M. T., Buckholtz, J. W., Schwartzman, A. N., Lambert, W. E., & Zald, D. H. (2009). Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS ONE, 4, e6598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trøstheim, M., Eikemo, M., Meir, R., Hansen, I., Paul, E., Kroll, S. L., … Leknes, S. (2020). Assessment of anhedonia in adults with and without mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American Medical Association JAMA Network Open, 3, e2013233.Google ScholarPubMed
Volkow, N. D., & McLellan, A. T. (2016). Opioid abuse in chronic pain – misconceptions and mitigation strategies. New England Journal of Medicine, 374, 12531263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vowles, K. E., McEntee, M. L., Julnes, P. S., Frohe, T., Ney, J. P., & van der Goes, D. N. (2015). Rates of opioid misuse, abuse, and addiction in chronic pain: A systematic review and data synthesis. PAIN, 156, 569576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J., & Zhou, R. (2014). Individual differences in automatic emotion regulation affect the asymmetry of the LPP component. Public Library of Science PLoS ONE, 9, e88261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics (N = 63) of patients with chronic pain on long-term opioid therapy (LTOT) treated with Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) or a supportive group (SG) psychotherapy control condition

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Changes in parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) during (a) (top left) passive viewing of natural reward (i.e. View trials) from pre- to post-treatment with Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE); (b) (top right) up-regulation of responding to natural reward (i.e. Regulate trials) from pre- to post-treatment with MORE; (c) (bottom left) passive viewing of natural reward (i.e. View trials) from pre- to post-treatment with supportive group (SG) psychotherapy; (d) (bottom right) up-regulation of responding to natural reward (i.e. Regulate trials) from pre- to post-treatment with SG.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Difference waves (post-treatment minus pre-treatment) depicting effects of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) response to natural reward cues, averaged across View and Regulate trials.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Difference waves (post-treatment minus pre-treatment) depicting effects of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on SCL response to natural reward cues, averaged across View and Regulate trials for 500 ms windows through the 5 s cue presentation.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Path model indicating that the effect of Mindfulness-Oriented Recovery Enhancement (MORE) v. supportive group (SG) psychotherapy on reducing anhedonia by 4-month follow-up was statistically mediated by increasing parietal (Pz) late positive potential (LPP) to natural reward cues.

Supplementary material: File

Garland et al. supplementary material

Garland et al. supplementary material

Download Garland et al. supplementary material(File)
File 62.7 KB