Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T12:38:31.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early predictors of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in assault survivors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 June 2007

BIRGIT KLEIM
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Psychology, King's College London, London, UK
ANKE EHLERS*
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, Department of Psychology, King's College London, London, UK
EDWARD GLUCKSMAN
Affiliation:
Accident and Emergency Department, King's College Hospital London, London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Professor Anke Ehlers, Department of Psychology (PO77), Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. (Email: anke.ehlers@iop.kcl.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Some studies suggest that early psychological treatment is effective in preventing chronic post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), but it is as yet unclear how best to identify trauma survivors who need such intervention. This prospective longitudinal study investigated the prognostic validity of acute stress disorder (ASD), of variables derived from a meta-analysis of risk factors for PTSD, and of candidate cognitive and biological variables in predicting chronic PTSD following assault.

Method

Assault survivors who had been treated for their injuries at a metropolitan Accident and Emergency (A&E) Department were assessed with structured clinical interviews to establish diagnoses of ASD at 2 weeks (n=222) and PTSD at 6 months (n=205) after the assault. Candidate predictors were assessed at 2 weeks.

Results

Most predictors significantly predicted PTSD status at follow-up. Multivariate logistic regressions showed that a set of four theory-derived cognitive variables predicted PTSD best (Nagelkerke R2=0·50), followed by the variables from the meta-analysis (Nagelkerke R2=0·37) and ASD (Nagelkerke R2=0·25). When all predictors were considered simultaneously, mental defeat, rumination and prior problems with anxiety or depression were chosen as the best combination of predictors (Nagelkerke R2=0·47).

Conclusion

Questionnaires measuring mental defeat, rumination and pre-trauma psychological problems may help to identify assault survivors at risk of chronic PTSD.

Type
Original Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

INTRODUCTION

After experiencing a violent traumatic event, such as an assault or terrorist attack, most people show some symptoms of distress, but only a minority develop persistent symptoms of sufficient severity to warrant a diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Galea et al. Reference Galea, Ahern, Resnick, Kilpatrick, Bucuvalas, Gold and Vlahov2002; Rubin et al. Reference Rubin, Brewin, Greenberg, Simpson and Wessely2005; Shalev & Freedman, Reference Shalev and Freedman2005). Most people recover from trauma without formal intervention (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes and Nelson1995), and there is evidence that brief early interventions given to all trauma survivors do not prevent chronic PTSD (for reviews, see NCCMH, 2005; Rose et al. Reference Rose, Bisson, Churchill and Wesseley2002). However, early trauma-focused cognitive-behavioural treatment of trauma survivors with high levels of PTSD symptoms is effective (for recent reviews, see NCCMH, 2005; Bisson & Cohen, Reference Bisson and Cohen2006), and this treatment may be even more effective when delivered within the first weeks post-trauma than at later time points (Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Freedman, Adessky and Watson2006). This has led to recommendations that early trauma-focused cognitive-behavioural treatment should be offered to trauma survivors with severe symptoms, and that screening should be considered for populations at risk of chronic PTSD (e.g. NCCMH, 2005).

This raises the question of how best to identify trauma survivors who are at risk of developing chronic PTSD and need intervention. One of the motivations for introducing the diagnosis of acute stress disorder (ASD) into DSM-IV (APA, 1994) was to help to identify, within the first weeks post-trauma, those individuals who are likely to go on to develop PTSD (Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Spitzer and Liebowitz1999). A range of empirical studies found indeed that the majority of trauma survivors with ASD go on to develop PTSD (e.g. Harvey & Bryant, Reference Harvey and Bryant1998; Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews, Rose and Kirk1999). However, several authors have recently questioned the predictive validity of the ASD diagnosis for subsequent PTSD, and have particularly challenged its sensitivity (McNally et al. Reference McNally, Bryant and Ehlers2003; Creamer et al. Reference Creamer, O'Donnell and Pattison2004). Studies with different trauma populations found that on average only 46% of those who developed PTSD had suffered from ASD in the initial month after trauma exposure (McNally et al. Reference McNally, Bryant and Ehlers2003); proportions ranged from 10% (severely injured accident survivors; Schnyder et al. Reference Schnyder, Moergli, Klaghofer and Buddeberg2001) to 72% (survivors of mild traumatic brain injury; Harvey & Bryant, Reference Harvey and Bryant2000). The low sensitivity poses a problem for clinical purposes as it may lead to a failure to identify individuals in need of treatment.

A review by Bryant (Reference Bryant2003) concluded that psychological or biological reactions in the acute trauma phase may provide more accurate means of predicting PTSD than symptom-based measures such as the ASD diagnosis, but this hypothesis remains to be tested. The present paper explored the question of whether risk factors identified in previous research may help to improve the early identification of trauma survivors who are likely to develop PTSD.

The first set of predictors under consideration was based on a meta-analysis of risk factors for PTSD (Ozer et al. Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003). The meta-analysis identified seven variables as the best empirically established predictors of PTSD; most of these had also emerged as risk factors in an earlier meta-analysis (Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000): peritraumatic dissociation (weighted effect size: r=0·35), perceived life threat during trauma, peritraumatic emotional responses (both r's=0·26), history of trauma, history of psychological problems prior to the trauma, family history of psychopathology (all r's=0·17), and post-trauma social support (r=−0·28). However, Ozer et al. (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) used the term ‘predictor’ in a statistical sense and included many cross-sectional studies, some of which were conducted many years the trauma. It remains to be tested whether the risk factors identified in their meta-analysis consistently predict chronic PTSD when evaluated in a prospective longitudinal design.

The second set of candidate predictors was derived from a cognitive model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). This model emphasizes factors that maintain PTSD in order to explain why some people develop persistent symptoms whereas others recover on their own. A series of prospective longitudinal studies showed that the cognitive factors specified in the model predict chronic PTSD at 6 months, 1 year and 3 years (Ehlers et al. Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant1998 b, Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant2003; Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Mayou et al. Reference Mayou, Ehlers and Bryant2002; Murray et al. Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Michael et al. Reference Michael, Ehlers, Halligan and Clark2005). The presumed maintaining factors also predicted PTSD severity at follow-up over and above what could be predicted from initial PTSD symptom severities. These include the candidate predictors chosen for the present study (with correlations with PTSD severity at 6 months to 1 year): mental defeat (Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; r=0·64), nowness of trauma memories (Michael et al. Reference Michael, Ehlers, Halligan and Clark2005; r=0·58), negative appraisals of the self, including negative interpretation of PTSD symptoms (Ehlers et al. 1998 b; Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; r's=0·43–0·62), and rumination about the trauma (Ehlers et al. 1998 b; r=0·48).

Finally, the study included resting heart rate as an easy-to-measure potential biological predictor that could be used in screening programmes. A recent review found that eight out of 10 studies showed that elevated heart rate in the acute trauma phase (measured at varying time points between admission to the emergency department and 1 month afterwards) is associated with later PTSD. Elevated heart rates may be a marker of a generalized fear response in those who are at risk of PTSD, and may reflect the strength of the noradrenergic response to the trauma (Bryant, Reference Bryant2006). However, there was great variability in heart rate levels and the strength of association with subsequent PTSD between studies (Bryant, Reference Bryant2006). Bryant et al. (Reference Bryant, Harvey, Guthrie and Moulds2000) found that a diagnosis of ASD or a resting heart rate of >90 beats/min (measured within 20 days post-trauma) predicted PTSD with high sensitivity (88%) and specificity (85%). Heart rate accounted for 4% of the variance of PTSD severity at follow-up.

The purpose of the present prospective longitudinal study was to compare the predictive power of the ASD diagnosis, the set of seven best predictors from Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis, the set of four theory-derived cognitive variables, and basal heart rate in the prediction of PTSD at 6 months. The 13 predictors were assessed at 2 weeks after injury in an assault. We investigated which constellation of variables would be most predictive of PTSD at 6 months.

METHOD

Sample characteristics

Participants had been injured in an assault and were recruited from consecutive attendees of King's College Hospital's Accident and Emergency (A&E) Department, London, UK. A total of 1063 assault survivors who attended the A&E department during the recruitment period (July 2003 to December 2004) were contacted, of whom 389 did not fulfil inclusion criteria and 197 declined to take part. Another 255 were initially interested, but failed to schedule or attend the research session within the designated time period. The remaining 222 assault survivors participated in a diagnostic session at 2 weeks, and 205 of these (92%) were interviewed again at 6 months. Exclusion criteria were current psychosis, assault occurring in the context of ongoing domestic violence, and inability to remember the assault (e.g. due to head injury). Assaults were mainly physical (99%), and 1% were sexual assaults. Demographic characteristics are listed in Table 1. The study sample was compared on demographic and assault characteristics with a random sample of identical size, drawn from 2785 assault survivors admitted to the same A&E department during a period of 1 year. There were no significant differences in age, sex or seriousness of injuries as measured by triage category. However, the study sample comprised more Caucasian participants compared to the random A&E sample, χ2(df=1)=17·88, p<0·001, 58·1% v. 36·9%Footnote .

Table 1. Demographic, clinical and assault characteristics for participants who completed assessments at both 2 weeks and 6 months (n=205)

s.d., Standard deviation.

Assault characteristics were missing for one participant.

a Combined household income.

Education correlated negatively with PTSD at 6 months (r=−0·25, p<0·001) and age correlated positively (r=0·15, p<0·05). None of the other demographic variables distinguished between participants with and without PTSD at 6 months (all p's >0·07). With respect to assault characteristics, the PTSD group reported less alcohol or drug consumption during the assault [χ2(1, n=204)=11·71, p=0·001], but the groups did not differ significantly in other variables (e.g. injury severity, assault duration, and proximity to victims' home).

Measures

Diagnostic interviews

ASD and PTSD diagnoses were established with standard structured clinical interviews: the Acute Stress Disorder Scale (Bryant & Harvey, Reference Bryant and Harvey2000) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996). All interviews were conducted by B.K. under the supervision of A.E. Inter-rater reliability was excellent (κ=0·97 for ASD, κ=0·82 for PTSD; based on n=56 interviews, two trained raters).

Predictors from Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis

Psychological problems prior to trauma

Participants reported whether or not they had had problems with anxiety or depression in the past. This self-report measure correlated significantly with the SCID assessment of a history of major depression (r=0·49, p<0·001).

Family history of psychological problems

Participants reported whether any of their close family members had ever had a problem with anxiety or depression.

Perceived social support following trauma

A modified subscale from the Crisis Support Scale (CSS; Joseph, Reference Joseph and Yule1999) assessed social support soon after the assault (α=0·75). Participants rated seven different aspects of social support (e.g. ‘Whenever you wanted to talk, how often was there someone willing to listen?’) on a scale from 1 (never) to 7 (always). An original item enquiring about personal contact with other trauma survivors was omitted because it did not seem to be relevant for the majority of assault survivors.

Perceived life threat

Participants rated how much they feared for their life during the assault, on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strongly). This rating has been shown to predict PTSD (Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003).

Peritraumatic emotional response

This was measured on a seven-item scale (Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; α=0·89). Participants rated the extent to which they had experienced the following emotions during the assault: terrified, ashamed, helpless, fearful, guilty, horrified, frightened, each on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strongly).

Peritraumatic dissociation

Dissociation during the assault was assessed with the State Dissociation Questionnaire (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; α=0·88), a nine-item scale assessing different aspects of dissociation such as derealization, depersonalization, detachment, altered time sense, emotional numbing, and reduction of awareness in surroundings. The scale has been shown to have good reliability and predictive validity in previous studies (e.g. Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Clark and Ehlers2002).

Theory-derived cognitive predictors

Four more variables were chosen on the basis of earlier studies to each represent one of the four hypothesized cognitive domains specified in Ehlers & Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model of PTSD. The model states that people with PTSD experience a sense of current threat that arises from two sources: (1) excessively negative appraisals of the trauma and/or its consequences, and (2) certain characteristics of the trauma memory that lead to easy cue-driven retrieval (i.e. unintentional triggering by matching stimuli) of aspects of the trauma memory. These memory characteristics are thought to result from (3) the quality of cognitive processing during the event. Finally, a range of (4) cognitive and behavioural strategies (e.g. rumination, thought suppression, excessive precautions, and ongoing dissociation) that individuals use to control the threat and symptoms are thought to maintain PTSD.

Negative appraisals of the self

Negative appraisals of the self were measured with the Negative Thoughts about the Self subscale of the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI; 21 items; α=0·93; Foa et al. Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999). The scale measures generalized negative appraisals of the trauma and its aftermath (e.g. ‘My reactions since the event mean that I am going crazy’, ‘I have permanently changed for the worse’, ‘I am a weak person’) and has been shown to have good reliability, convergent validity and to discriminate well between traumatized people with and without PTSD (Foa et al. Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999).

‘Nowness’ of trauma memories

Trauma memories are thought to be disjointed from other autobiographical memories, leading to a deficit in the awareness that they reflect an experience of the self in the past (Ehlers et al. Reference Ehlers, Hackmann and Michael2004). Perceived ‘nowness’ of traumatic memories was measured with the corresponding item from the Assault Memory Questionnaire (Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003): ‘When I remember the assault, it is like happening again, here and now’. Self-reported ‘nowness’ correlates well with interview assessment (r=0·84; Hackmann et al. Reference Hackmann, Ehlers, Speckens and Clark2004), and the retest reliability is r=0·68 (Speckens et al. Reference Speckens, Ehlers, Hackmann and Clark2006).

Mental defeat

Mental defeat refers to processing the trauma as a complete loss of personal autonomy and has been shown to predict PTSD in survivors of assault and torture (Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers1999, Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Ehlers et al. Reference Ehlers, Maercker and Boos2000). The Mental Defeat Scale (Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers1999, Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; α=0·90) is an 11-item self-report questionnaire. Participants rated the extent to which statements such as ‘I no longer felt like a human being’ or ‘In my mind, I gave up’ applied to them at some time during the assault, each on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very strongly).

Rumination

Rumination about the trauma and its consequences was measured with the eight-item subscale of the Response to Intrusions Questionnaire (Clohessy & Ehlers, Reference Clohessy and Ehlers1999; Murray et al. Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; α=0·84). Participants rated how often they did things such as ‘I think about why the event happened to me’ or ‘I go over and over the assault’, when memories of the assault popped into their mind, each on a scale from 0 (never) to 3 (always). The scale has been shown to have good reliability and predictive validity (Murray et al. Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; Ehring et al. Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2006).

Resting heart rate

Heart inter-beat intervals were registered with an S810 Polar watch (Polar Electro, Vantaa, Finland) for a period of 3 min, while participants remained seated. Participants were instructed to sit quietly, not to speak and to keep their eyes open. Heart rate data were available for 177 participants (80%). The Polar device has been shown to reliably assess inter-beat intervals during rest as well as activity (e.g. Laukkanen & Virtanen, Reference Laukkanen and Virtanen1998). None of the participants were taking beta-blockers or other medication affecting the cardiovascular system.

Procedure

The study was approved by the local ethics committees, and participants gave written consent. Participants completed a research session comprising the completion of questionnaires assessing the psychological predictor variables, heart rate assessment and the ASD Scale (Bryant & Harvey, Reference Bryant and Harvey2000) interview. At 6 months, the SCID (First et al. Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1996) was conducted over the telephone by the same interviewer. Participants received £50 as reimbursement for their time.

Statistical analysis

SPSS version 13.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for most analyses; bootstrap analyses were implemented by Stata Statistics and Data Analysis Package version 9.0 (Stata Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). Predictors were z-standardized to allow direct comparison of odds ratios (ORs). Association between candidate predictors and PTSD were first investigated with univariate logistic regression analyses. Hierarchical logistic regression analysis tested which of the predictors explained PTSD at 6 months over and above what could be predicted from the ASD diagnosis, by forcing ASD into the equation in the first step and the predictor in the second step. In addition, two multivariate logistic regression analyses tested the predictive power of the variable sets from Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis and from the cognitive model (including only significant univariate predictors). Finally, a logistic regression analysis of all significant univariate predictors identified the combination of predictors that best predicted PTSD at 6 months; this model determined which variables to add to the model based on the likelihood ratio test. Reliability of univariate results was established by calculating confidence intervals (CIs) for ORs of all predictors with bootstrap resampling (Efron & Tibshirani, Reference Efron and Tibshirani1986), which involved drawing 1000 random subsamples of the total sample.

RESULTS

ASD and PTSD prevalence

At 2 weeks, 17% of the participants (n=37/222) met diagnostic criteria for ASD, and at 6 months, 24% (n=49/205) met diagnostic criteria for PTSD.

Early predictors of chronic PTSD

Table 2 shows the associations between the predictors assessed at 2 weeks and PTSD at 6 months. Most univariate associations were significant. Participants with ASD were at increased risk for PTSD at 6 months compared to those without ASD (non-transformed OR 11·7, φ=0·46). Table 2 shows that although the majority of participants with ASD went on to develop chronic PTSD (68%), ASD had low sensitivity (47%) in predicting PTSD.

Table 2. Associations of predictors at 2 weeks with PTSD diagnosis at 6 months

PTSD, Post-traumatic stress disorder; ASD, acute stress disorder; s.d., standard deviation; n.a., not applicable; CI, confidence interval.

a Predictors are z-standardized to make odds ratios comparable; b as non-significant in univariate analysis.

Questionnaire ranges: mental defeat 0–4, nowness of trauma memories 0–4, negative appraisals about the self 1–7, rumination 0–4, perceived life threat 0–4, perceived support 1–7, peritraumatic dissociation 0–4.

*** p<0·001, ** p<0·01, * p<0·05.

Most of the predictors derived from Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis were significantly associated with a greater PTSD risk (prior psychological problems, less post-trauma social support, greater perceived threat to life, peritraumatic emotional responses and dissociation during the assault). Associations with prior trauma (non-transformed OR 4·54, 95% CI 0·58–35·45, φ=0·11) and family history of psychological problems (non-transformed OR 1·36, 95% CI 0·66–2·82, φ=0·06) did not reach significance. All theory-derived cognitive variables (mental defeat, nowness of memories, negative appraisals of the self, and rumination about the trauma) and resting heart rate predicted PTSD. Bootstrap analyses for 1000 subsamples showed that ORs and CIs replicated well. Table 2 also shows which of the variables predicted PTSD at 6 months over and above what could be predicted from an ASD diagnosis at 2 weeks. These were the four theory-derived cognitive variables, prior psychological problems, perceived life threat, peritraumatic emotional response and dissociation, and resting heart rate.

Next, we compared the predictive power of the sets of variables under investigation (Table 2). The theory-derived cognitive variables explained 50% of the variance (sensitivity 0·60, specificity 0·95, 88% correct classifications). CIs indicated that the strength of the prediction was significantly larger than the 25% explained by an ASD diagnosis (sensitivity 0·47, specificity 0·93, 82% correct classifications), and the 4% explained by resting heart rate at 2 weeks (sensitivity 0·00, specificity 1·00). The established predictors identified in Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis explained 37% of the variance (sensitivity 0·44, specificity 0·95, 85% correct classifications), which was not significantly different from the predictive power of the ASD diagnosis.

When all significant predictors were considered in an overall multiple logistic regression analysis, mental defeat (OR 2·07, p=0·014, 95% CI 1·16–3·70), rumination about the trauma (OR 2·99, p=0·002, 95% CI 1·50–5·96) and prior psychological problems (OR 1·95, p=0·014, 95% CI 1·14–3·31) were chosen as the best combination of predictors by the forward likelihood ratio test (χ2=47·37, p=0·000). Together, the three variables explained 47% of the variance (sensitivity 0·57, specificity 0·96) and classified 88% of the cases correctly into PTSD and non-PTSD status. The Hosmer and Lemeshow goodness-of-fit test showed that the model fit the data well (χ2=9·86, p=0·275). Not selected were ASD (after last step p=0·184), perceived threat to life (p=0·452), peritraumatic dissociation (p=0·941), peritraumatic emotional response (p=0·695), nowness of trauma memories (p=0·138), negative appraisals about the self (p=0·589) and resting heart rate (p=0·828).

DISCUSSION

This prospective longitudinal study found that 17% of 222 injured assault survivors met diagnostic criteria for ASD at 2 weeks after the event, and at 6 months, 24% had PTSD. These prevalences are similar to those reported in other studies of assault survivors (e.g. Harvey & Bryant, Reference Harvey and Bryant1998; Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews, Rose and Kirk1999). While the majority of participants with ASD went on to develop chronic PTSD (68%), ASD had low sensitivity (47%) in predicting PTSD, again in accord with previous studies (Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews, Rose and Kirk1999; Creamer et al. Reference Creamer, O'Donnell and Pattison2004; Elklit & Brink, Reference Elklit and Brink2004; see McNally et al. Reference McNally, Bryant and Ehlers2003 for a review). One of the reasons for the low sensitivity of ASD may be that not all assault survivors at risk for PTSD may have had the three dissociative symptoms required for a diagnosis. Overall, the usefulness of ASD as a criterion for identifying people at risk of PTSD was limited.

This raises the question of whether there are other ways of identifying people at risk for chronic PTSD (e.g. Shalev et al. Reference Shalev, Freedman, Peri, Brandes and Sahar1997; Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Rose, Andrews, Green, Tata, McEvedy, Turner and Foa2002). The present study tested three groups of candidate predictors of PTSD identified in previous research, namely the seven best established predictors of PTSD according to the meta-analysis of Ozer et al. (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003), four predictors derived from a cognitive model of PTSD (Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000), and resting heart rate as a candidate biological predictor. In line with previous findings, nearly all of these variables predicted PTSD at 6 months (Ehlers et al. 1998 b; Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000; Bryant et al. Reference Bryant, Harvey, Guthrie and Moulds2000; Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Murray et al. Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; Halligan et al. Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Ozer et al. Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003). The results extend previous findings in several ways. (1) They demonstrated that the predictors not only are associated with concurrent PTSD but also predict chronic PTSD prospectively; this strengthens Ozer et al.'s (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) findings, which were based largely on cross-sectional studies. (2) The variables were predictive even when assessed 2 weeks after the trauma, which extends the results of previous prospective longitudinal studies where the initial assessment took place later (e.g. Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001). (3) The study allowed a direct comparison of the strength of the prediction of different sets of variables suggested as relevant in the literature. (4) Recruitment from an A&E department ensured a sample that was representative in terms of the severity of assaults and most other demographic characteristics.

Overall, the results replicated well those of the Ozer et al. (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis. As in the meta-analysis, peritraumatic variables (peritraumatic dissociation and emotions, perceived life threat) and social support tended to be stronger predictors than pre-trauma characteristics (see also Brewin et al. Reference Brewin, Andrews and Valentine2000). The finding that the relationships between prior trauma and a family history of psychological problems and PTSD did not reach significance in this study should not be overinterpreted. Given the modest PTSD rate in the sample, our study had low power for detecting very small effect sizes. Restrictions in range may have played a role as the majority of the participants reported prior trauma. However, it is also possible that the type of trauma or timing of assessment plays a role in the strength of the relationship of these variables with PTSD, as other studies of single-event trauma have also failed to establish a relationship between family history and PTSD (Blanchard & Hickling, Reference Blanchard and Hickling2004) and as Ozer et al. (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) identified three previous studies of recent trauma survivors (⩽4 months), all of which found no relationship between trauma history and PTSD (r's=0·00–0·03).

The set of predictors from the Ozer et al. (Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003) meta-analysis did not significantly predict chronic PTSD better than the ASD diagnosis. A possible explanation may be that these variables mainly predict the onset of symptoms, but are less useful for the prediction of long-term outcome as maintaining factors are not included (e.g. Ehlers & Steil, Reference Ehlers and Steil1995). By contrast, the theory-derived cognitive factors each predicted PTSD at 6 months over and above what can be predicted from an ASD diagnosis at 2 weeks, and together explained more variance than ASD. This finding may reflect the fact that PTSD symptoms show substantial natural recovery in the first months after trauma (e.g. Rothbaum et al. Reference Rothbaum, Foa, Riggs, Murdock and Walsh1992; Kessler et al. Reference Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes and Nelson1995), and factors that impede recovery and maintain PTSD symptoms may therefore be of particular interest in predicting chronic PTSD (e.g. Ehlers & Steil, Reference Ehlers and Steil1995; Ehlers & Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000).

The logistic regression analysis that considered all candidate predictors simultaneously identified a combination of two of such theory-derived maintaining variables (mental defeat, rumination) and a vulnerability variable (psychological problems prior to the assault) as the best predictor set. This suggests that the combination of vulnerability and maintaining factors may provide the best prediction of chronic PTSD. The combination of these three predictors predicted diagnostic status at 6 months correctly in 88% of the cases. Together, the three variables explained almost half of the variance in PTSD diagnosis at 6 months, compared to 25% of explained variance by the ASD diagnosis.

In line with previous research, resting heart rate at 2 weeks predicted PTSD at 6 months, but only explained a small proportion of the variance. Bryant (Reference Bryant2006) proposed multiple pathways to PTSD development, some of which are characterized by elevated and others by lowered heart rate levels (alongside dissociative responses). Such differences in response pattern would be consistent with the present results, but limit the usefulness of basal heart rate as a way of early screening for risk of chronic PTSD. Heart rate may be more predictive if it is assessed sooner after the trauma, although the currently available data show great variation for the predictive power of such early assessments (Bryant, Reference Bryant2006). It is also possible that the participants' heart rate may have been influenced by memories triggered by the questionnaires that were part of the research session, although such responses would have been expected to increase rather than decrease the relationship between heart rate and PTSD.

The study had strengths and limitations. Among the strength are the prospective longitudinal design, the relatively large number of participants, the excellent follow-up rate, the use of reliable structured interviews to establish diagnoses, the replication of the results in bootstrapping analyses, and the early initial assessment. Nevertheless, further research will need to establish how well the present results generalize to other populations with different PTSD rates and other forms of trauma before the results are applied to clinical practice. The sample showed a moderate PTSD rate of 24% at follow-up, and had low power to detect very small effect sizes. Thirteen potential predictors were considered, which raises the possibility of false positive associations. However, all of the predictors had been shown to be associated with PTSD risk in previous studies, and the present sample replicated earlier results well and showed good replication in bootstrapping analyses. Although our analyses indicated that the sample was fairly representative of assault survivors attending the A&E department, it comprised relatively fewer people of ethnic minorities compared to the population of assault survivors treated at this hospital (nevertheless, the 42% exceeded the 24% ethnic minorities living in London according to the 2001 Census). The concept of mental defeat, one of the strongest predictors in this study, was developed with survivors of interpersonal violence such as assault and torture (e.g. Dunmore et al. Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers1999, Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Ehlers et al. 1998 a, Reference Ehlers, Maercker and Boos2000), and other predictors may be more relevant for survivors of other traumas such as accidents or natural disaster. A further possible limitation was that PTSD diagnoses at 6 months were based on a diagnostic interview conducted over the telephone, rather than a face-to-face diagnostic interview. It may be argued that telephone interviews make participants somewhat less forthcoming in providing information about their symptoms than face-to-face interviews. However, the interviewer had already established a relationship with the participants at the first assessment, which makes it unlikely that participants withheld information from her later. Finally, personal and family history of problems with anxiety or depression was assessed with simple self-report questions, and more sophisticated assessments may have given different results.

In conclusion, this study found that PTSD at 6 months after injury in an assault can be predicted from early assessment at 2 weeks. A set of four theory-derived cognitive variables predicted PTSD better than the ASD diagnosis. Logistic regression analysis suggested that the combination of mental defeat, rumination and a history of problems with anxiety or depression predicted PTSD best. These concepts can be measured with only 20 self-report questions. However, it remains to be tested how the three selected scales perform on their own, as they were selected from a larger group of 11 significant predictors in this study. If replicated, our results suggest that assault survivors are at heightened PTSD risk if they report mental defeat at the time of the event, if they ruminate about the trauma, and if they report prior problems with depression or anxiety. Early assessment of these predictors appears straightforward and may be useful for screening programmes in general practice or communities affected by trauma. They may help to identify those trauma survivors who are most likely to develop PTSD and who would benefit from early intervention. Putting the experience of mental defeat in perspective and reducing rumination appear to be promising targets for such interventions.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was funded by a grant from the Psychiatry Research Trust and a Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellowship to Anke Ehlers. We thank Thomas Ehring, Silke Frank, Inga Boellinghaus, Emma Briddon, Anke Weidmann, Ines Sengstock, Johanna Hissbach, Jennifer Baumeister, Stephanie Spengler and the staff of King's College Accident and Emergency Department for their help.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None.

Footnotes

The figure of 36·9% presents an underestimate of the number of Caucasian participants. It is based on A&E department codes that require the staff to code ethnicity in 80 categories based on country of origin. In practice, the category of ‘other’ was often used for non-British participants regardless of their ethnic background.

References

REFERENCES

APA (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Bisson, J. I. & Cohen, J. A. (2006). Disseminating early interventions following trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress 19, 583595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, E. B. & Hickling, E. J. (2004). After the Crash: Psychological Assessment and Treatment of Survivors of Motor Vehicle Accidents (2nd edn). American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., Rose, S. & Kirk, M. (1999). Acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in victims of violent crime. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 360366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B. & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68, 748766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. R., Rose, S., Andrews, B., Green, J., Tata, P., McEvedy, C., Turner, S. & Foa, E. B. (2002). Brief screening instrument for post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry 181, 158162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A. (2003). Early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry 53, 789795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A. (2006). Longitudinal psychophysiological studies of heart rate: mediating effects and implications for treatment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1071, 1926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A. & Harvey, A. G. (2000). Acute Stress Disorder: A Handbook of Theory, Assessment, and Treatment. American Psychological Association: Washington, DC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, R. A., Harvey, A. G., Guthrie, R. M. & Moulds, M. L. (2000). A prospective study of psychophysiological arousal, acute stress disorder, and posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 341344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clohessy, S. & Ehlers, A. (1999). PTSD symptoms, response to intrusive memories and coping in ambulance service workers. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 38, 251265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creamer, M., O'Donnell, M. L. & Pattison, P. (2004). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in severely injured trauma survivors. Behaviour Research and Therapy 43, 315328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M. & Ehlers, A. (1999). Cognitive factors involved in the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy 37, 809829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunmore, E., Clark, D. M. & Ehlers, A. (2001). A prospective investigation of the role of cognitive factors in persistent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 10631084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. (1986). Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals and other measures of statistical accuracy. Statistical Science 1, 5477.Google Scholar
Ehlers, A. & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 38, 319345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Dunmore, E., Jaycox, L., Meadows, E. & Foa, E. B. (1998 a). Predicting response to exposure treatment in PTSD: the role of mental defeat and alienation. Journal of Traumatic Stress 11, 457471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A. & Michael, T. (2004). Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: phenomenology, theory, and therapy. Memory 12, 403415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Maercker, A. & Boos, A. (2000). Posttraumatic stress disorder following political imprisonment: the role of mental defeat, alienation, and perceived permanent change. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 4555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A. & Bryant, B. (1998 b). Psychological predictors of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 508519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A., Mayou, R. A. & Bryant, B. (2003). Cognitive predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder in children: results of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 41, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlers, A. & Steil, R. (1995). Maintenance of intrusive memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a cognitive approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy 23, 217249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehring, T., Ehlers, A. & Glucksman, E. (2006). Contribution of cognitive factors to the prediction of post-traumatic stress disorder, phobia and depression after motor vehicle accidents. Behaviour Research and Therapy 44, 16991716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elklit, A. & Brink, O. (2004). Acute stress disorder as a predictor of post-traumatic stress disorder in physical assault victims. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 19, 709726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M. & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., Tolin, D. F. & Orsillo, S. M. (1999). The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): development and validation. Psychological Assessment 11, 303314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., Kilpatrick, D., Bucuvalas, M., Gold, J. & Vlahov, D. (2002). Psychological sequelae of the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. New England Journal of Medicine 346, 982987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., Speckens, A. & Clark, D. M. (2004). Characteristics and content of intrusive memories in PTSD and their changes with treatment. Journal of Traumatic Stress 17, 231240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, S. L., Clark, D. M. & Ehlers, A. (2002). Cognitive processing, memory, and the development of PTSD symptoms: two experimental analogue studies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 33, 7389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halligan, S. L., Michael, T., Clark, D. M. & Ehlers, A. (2003). Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: the role of cognitive processing, trauma memory, and appraisals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 71, 419431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G. & Bryant, R. A. (1998). The relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: a prospective evaluation of motor vehicle accident survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 66, 507512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, A. G. & Bryant, R. A. (2000). A two-year prospective evaluation of the relationship between acute stress disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder following mild traumatic brain injury. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 626628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, S. (1999). Social support and mental health following trauma. In Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Concepts and Therapy (ed. Yule, W.), pp. 7191. Wiley: Chichester.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M. & Nelson, C. B. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 52, 10481060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukkanen, R. M. T. & Virtanen, P. (1998). Heart rate monitors: state of the art. Journal of Sports Sciences 16, 37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, R. D., Spitzer, R. & Liebowitz, M. R. (1999). Review and critique of the new DSM-IV diagnosis of acute stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 16771685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayou, R. A., Ehlers, A. & Bryant, B. (2002). Posttraumatic stress disorder after motor vehicle accidents: 3-year follow-up of a prospective longitudinal study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 40, 665675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, R. J., Bryant, R. A. & Ehlers, A. (2003). Does early psychological intervention promote recovery from posttraumatic stress? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 4, 4579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michael, T., Ehlers, A., Halligan, S. & Clark, D. M. (2005). Unwanted memories of assault: what intrusion characteristics predict PTSD? Behaviour Research and Therapy 43, 613628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, J., Ehlers, A. & Mayou, R. A. (2002). Dissociation and post-traumatic stress disorder: two prospective studies of road traffic accident survivors. British Journal of Psychiatry 180, 363368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NCCMH (2005). Post-traumatic stress disorder: the management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary care. National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health. National Institute for Clinical Excellence (NICE): London, UK.Google Scholar
Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L. & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 129, 5273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S., Bisson, J., Churchill, R. & Wesseley, S. (2002). Psychological debriefing for preventing posttraumatic stress disorder. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Issue 2. Article No.: CD00560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbaum, B. O., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Murdock, T. & Walsh, W. (1992). A prospective examination of posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims. Journal of Traumatic Stress 5, 455475.Google Scholar
Rubin, G. J., Brewin, C. R., Greenberg, N., Simpson, J. & Wessely, S. (2005). Psychological and behavioural reactions to the bombings in London on 7 July 2005: cross sectional survey of a representative sample of Londoners. British Medical Journal 331, 606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnyder, U., Moergli, H., Klaghofer, R. & Buddeberg, C. (2001). Incidence and prediction of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in severely injured accident victims. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 594599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, A. Y. & Freedman, S. (2005). PTSD following terrorist attacks: a prospective evaluation. American Journal of Psychiatry 170, 558564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shalev, A. Y., Freedman, S., Adessky, R. & Watson, P. (2006). Who needs care, who wants care, who is helped by early intervention: 5600 trauma survivors' results. Abstract no. 159877. In Symposium at the 22nd Annual Meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, Hollywood, CA, USA.Google Scholar
Shalev, A. Y., Freedman, S., Peri, T., Brandes, D. & Sahar, T. (1997). Predicting PTSD in trauma survivors: prospective evaluation of self-report and clinician-administered instruments. British Journal of Psychiatry 170, 558564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Speckens, A. E., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A. & Clark, D. M. (2006). Changes in intrusive memories associated with imaginal reliving in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 20, 328341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic, clinical and assault characteristics for participants who completed assessments at both 2 weeks and 6 months (n=205)

Figure 1

Table 2. Associations of predictors at 2 weeks with PTSD diagnosis at 6 months