Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T10:44:35.426Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Affective Coherence

Affect as Embodied Evidence in Attitude, Advertising, and Art

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Gün R. Semin
Affiliation:
Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam
Eliot R. Smith
Affiliation:
Indiana University, Bloomington
Gerald L. Clore
Affiliation:
University of Virginia, VA, USA
Simone Schnall
Affiliation:
University of Plymouth
Get access

Summary

Existence precedes and rules essence.

Sartre, J. P. (1943). Being and Nothingness

In this chapter, we review behavioral research connecting affective embodiment and evaluative cognition. We argue that affect and belief exist in a dynamic relationship. Evaluative beliefs elicit affective experience, and affective experience provides data for evaluative conception. We propose that such reciprocal relationships exist because emotions are, in part, embodiments of evaluation. That is, emotions exist when the same goodness or badness is represented simultaneously in multiple systems. Indeed, emotion is arguably nothing but the co-occurrence of evaluation in thought, feeling, physiology, expression, and so on. The embodied nature of some of these representations makes evaluative beliefs especially compelling. Thus, in contrast to a mere idea that something may be good or bad in some way, an emotion is an embodied commitment to such a reality.

Our general research program focuses on the idea that embodied affective reactions act as critical information for making judgments, guiding cognitive processing, and selecting events to remember. However, in this chapter we focus on how such embodiment serves to validate or invalidate evaluative beliefs. We also suggest that the impact of an idea in art often depends on its ability to elicit embodied reactions, that is, its ability to move people.

The experience of emotion has two discriminable components – valence and arousal (Barrett, this volume; Russell, 2003). In turn, these components are embodied representations of value and importance (Clore & Schnall, 2005).

Type
Chapter
Information
Embodied Grounding
Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches
, pp. 211 - 236
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adaval, R. (2001). Sometimes it just feels right: The differential weighting of affect-consistent and affect-inconsistent information. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 1–17.Google Scholar
Allen, J., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (pp. 319–335). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (1996). How emotion helps and hurts decision making. In Forgas, J. (Ed.), Hearts and Minds: Affective influences on social cognition and behaviour. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Bernston, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determination of attitudes. II. Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 294–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centerbar, D., & Clore, G. L. (2006). Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes?Psychological Science, 17, 22–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centerbar, D. B., Schnall, S., Clore, G. L., & Garvin, E. (in press). Affective incoherence: When affective concepts and embodied reactions clash. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Gasper, K. (2000). Feeling is believing: Some affective influences on belief. In Frijda, N. H., Manstead, A. S. R., & Bem, S. (Eds.), Emotions and beliefs: How do emotions influence beliefs? (pp. 10–44). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognitive in emotion: Never, sometimes, or always? In Nadel, L. & Lane, R. (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24–61). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., & Schnall, S. (2005). The influences of affect on attitude. In Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes (pp. 437–490). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of emotion in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872).Google Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., Bargh, J. A., & Miedema, J. (2000). Of men and mackerels: Attention and automatic behavior. In Bless, H. & Forgas, J. P. (Eds.), Subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 36–51). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion extraversion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality. In Eysenck, H. J. (Ed.), A model for personality (pp. 246–276). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in cognition: The Implicit Association Task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1469–1480.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 179–236). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 1–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Kubovy, M. (1986). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Meier, B. P., & Robinson, M. D. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on color. Psychological Science, 15, 82–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monteith, M. J., Voils, C. I., & Ashburn-Nardo, L. (2001). Taking a look underground: Detecting, interpreting, and reacting to implicit racial biases. Social Cognition, 19, 395–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press (reprinted 1999).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perrine, L. (1956). Sound and sense: An introduction to poetry. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one's cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 410–424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, M. D., Vargas, P., & Crawford, E. G. (2003). Putting process into personality, appraisal, and emotion: Evaluative processing as a missing link. In Musch, J. & Klauer, C. (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 275–306). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well- being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1988). How do I feel about it? The informative function of mood. In Fiedler, K. & Forgas, J. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 44–62). Toronto: C. J. Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. (Eds.), Social psychology. A handbook of basic principles 2nd ed. (pp. 385–407). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Shields, B. (2005). Down came the rain: My journey through postpartum depression. New York: Hyperion Books.Google Scholar
Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2004). Socially situated cognition: Cognition in its social context. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 53–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). The epistemic benefits of trait-consistent mood states: An analysis of extraversion and mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 663–677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamir, M., Robinson, M. D., Clore, G. L., Martin, L. L., & Whitaker, D. J. (2004). Are we puppets on a string? The contextual meaning of unconscious expressive cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 237–249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargas, P. (2004). Implicit and explicit measures of sexual orientation: You may not be as straight as you think you are, and it's making you sick. Paper presented at the meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology (Conference on Attitudes), Madrid, Spain, June 2004.
Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap/Harvard Press.Google Scholar
Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation leads to positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 989–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, H. (1984) Affect and cognition: The hard interface. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior (pp. 73–102). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×