Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T11:48:23.584Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - The Fröhlich effect: past and present

from Part IV - Spatial phenomena: forward shift effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2010

Romi Nijhawan
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Beena Khurana
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Summary

When observers are asked to localize the initial position of a moving target, they often indicate a position displaced in the direction of motion relative to the true onset position. In this review, the debate between Fröhlich, who discovered this phenomenon, and his contemporaries in the 1920s and 1930s is summarized. Striking misinterpretations of Fröhlich's findings and the anticipation of recent research on the flash-lag effect will be presented. In the second part, current accounts of the Fröhlich effect in terms of attention and metacontrast are evaluated. In the final section, reconciliation between research on the Fröhlich effect and recent reports of an error opposite the direction of motion (the onset repulsion effect) is offered.

Introduction

When asked to localize a moving target entering a window, observers often indicate a position not adjacent to the edge of the window but a position displaced in the direction of motion (see Fig. 19.1(a)). The gap between the edge of a window and the initial perception of the moving target was first discovered by the Norwegian astronomer O. Pihl in 1894, but Fröhlich (1923) was the first to study the effect systematically. Therefore, the illusion has been named the “Fröhlich effect.” Fröhlich's explanation of the illusion in terms of “sensation time” was amply discussed in the 1930s (Fröhlich 1930, 1932; Rubin 1930; G. E. Müller 1931; Metzger 1932; Piéron 1935) but forgotten for the 60 years that followed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Actis-Grosso, R., & Stucchi, N. (2003). Shifting the start: backward mislocation of the initial position of a motion. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29(3): 675–691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alpern, M. (1953). Metacontrast. J Opt Soc Am 43(8): 648–657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldo, M. V., & Klein, S. A. (1995). Extrapolation or attention shift? Nature 378(6557): 565–566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breitmeyer, B., & Öğmen, H. (2006). Visual Masking: Time Slices Through Conscious and Unconscious Vision. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burr, D. (1980). Motion smear. Nature 284(5752): 164–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, R. (2003). The Fröhlich effect is not due to a failure to perceive the beginning portion of motion trajectory. J Vis 3(9): 485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, M. (1980). Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics 27(3): 183–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (2000). Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes. J Exp Psychol Gen 129(4): 481–507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagleman, D. M., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science 287(5460): 2036–2038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, C. W., & Murphy, T. D. (1987). Movement of attentional focus across the visual field: a critical look at the evidence. Perception & Psychophysics 42(3): 299–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, G., & Hermens, F. (2002). Comment on “Competition for consciousness among visual events: the psychophysics of reentrant visual processes” (Di Lollo, Enns, & Rensink, 2000). J Exp Psychol Gen 131(4): 590–593; discussion 594–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, F. W. (1923). Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit. [On the measurement of sensation time]. Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie 54: 58–78.Google Scholar
Fröhlich, F. W. (1930). Über die Messung der Empfindungszeit. Eine Erwiderung auf experimenteller Grundlage [On the measurement of sensation time. An answer on an experimental basis]. Psychologische Forschung 13: 285–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fröhlich, F. W. (1932). Bemerkungen zu G.E. Müllers Kritik der Empfindungszeitmessung [Remarks on the criticism of G. E. Müller of the measurement of sensation time]. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane 62: 246–249.Google Scholar
Geer, M., & Schmidt, W. C. (2006). Perception of initial moving target signals: support for a cumulative lateral inhibition theory. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(5): 1185–1196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. L., & Motes, M. A. (2002). Does representational momentum reflect a distortion of the length or the endpoint of a trajectory? Cognition 82(3): B89–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. L., & Motes, M. A. (2005). An effect of context on whether memory for initial position exhibits a Fröhlich Effect or an Onset Repulsion Effect. Q J Exp Psychol 58A(6): 961–979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerzel, D. (2002). Different localization of motion onset with pointing and relative judgements. Exp Brain Res 145(3): 340–350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerzel, D. (2004). Attentional load modulates mislocalization of moving stimuli, but does not eliminate the error. Psychon Bull Rev 11(5): 848–853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerzel, D., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2004). Spatial distortions and processing latencies in the onset repulsion and Fröhlich effects. Vision Res 44(6): 577–590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerzel, D., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Effects of stimulus material on the Fröhlich illusion. Vision Res 42(2): 181–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khurana, B., & Nijhawan, R. (1995). Extrapolation or attention shift. Nature 378(6557): 566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khurana, B., Watanabe, K., & Nijhawan, R. (2000). The role of attention in motion extrapolation: are moving objects “corrected” or flashed objects attentionally delayed? Perception 29(6): 675–692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschfeld, K., & Kammer, T. (1999). The Fröhlich effect: a consequence of the interaction of visual focal attention and metacontrast. Vision Res 39: 3702–3709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreegipuu, K., & Allik, J. (2003). Perceived onset time and position of a moving stimulus. Vision Res 43(15): 1625–1635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKay, D. M. (1958). Perceptual stability of a stroboscopically lit visual field containing self-luminous objects. Nature 181(4607): 507–508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzger, W. (1932). Versuch einer gemeinsamen Theorie der Phänomene Fröhlichs und Hazelhoffs und Kritik ihrer Verfahren zur Messung der Empfindungszeit [An attempt at a common theory of Fröhlich's and Hazelhoff's phenomena and a critique of their procedure for measuring sensation time]. Psychologische Forschung 16: 176–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, G. E. (1931). Erklärung der Erscheinungen eines mit konstanter Geschwindigkeit beewegten Lichtstreifens, insbesondere auch des Pihl-Fröhlichschen Phänomens [Explanation of the appearances of a lit bar moving at constant velocity, in particular the phenomenon of Pihl-Fröhlich]. Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie 62: 167–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15(2): 315–330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müsseler, J., & Aschersleben, G. (1998). Localizing the first position of a moving stimulus: the Fröhlich effect and an attention-shifting explanation. Perception & Psychophysics 60(4): 683–695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müsseler, J., & Kerzel, D. (2004). The trial context determines adjusted localization of stimuli: reconciling the Fröhlich and Onset Repulsion Effects. Vision Res 44(19): 2201–2206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müsseler, J., & Neumann, O. (1992). Apparent distance reduction with moving stimuli (Tandem Effect): evidence for an attention-shifting model. Psychol Res 54(4): 246–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müsseler, J., Stork, S., & Kerzel, D. (2002). Comparing mislocalizations with moving stimuli. The Fröhlich effect, the flash-lag effect and representational momentum. Vis Cogn 9(1/2): 120–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijhawan, R. (1994). Motion extrapolation in catching. Nature 370(6487): 256–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijhawan, R., Watanabe, K., Khurana, B., & Shimojo, S. (2004). Compensation of neural delays in visual-motor behaviour: no evidence for shorter afferent delays for visual motion. Vis Cogn 11(2/3): 275–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piéron, H. (1935). Le processus du métacontraste. Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 32: 1–24.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32(1): 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H., Bouma & D. G., Bouwhuis (eds.), Attention and Performance X. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol 109(2): 160–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purushothaman, G., Patel, S. S., Bedell, H. E., & Öğmen, H. (1998). Moving ahead through differential visual latency. Nature 396(6710): 424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Remington, R., & Pierce, L. (1984). Moving attention: evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics 35(4): 393–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, E. (1930). Kritisches und Experimentelles zur “Empfindungszeit” Fröhlichs [Critical and experimental remarks on Fröhlich's “Sensation Time]. Psychologische Forschung 13(101–112).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runeson, S. (1974). Constant velocity: not perceived as such. Psychol Res 37(1): 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: past, present, and future. Trends Cogn Sci 9(1): 16–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stigler, R. (1910). Chronophotische Studien über den Umgebungskontrast. Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie 134: 365–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, I. M. (2002). The onset repulsion effect. Spat Vis 15(2): 219–244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, D., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). The position of moving objects. Science 289(5482): 1107a.Google ScholarPubMed
Whitney, D., & Cavanagh, P. (2002). Surrounding motion affects the perceived locations of moving stimuli. Vis Cogn 9(1/2): 139–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, D., & Murakami, I. (1998). Latency difference, not spatial extrapolation. Nat Neurosci 1(8): 656–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×