Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T15:13:21.704Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Autobiographical memories and beliefs: a preliminary metacognitive model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Giuliana Mazzoni
Affiliation:
Seton Hall University, USA
Irving Kirsh
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut, USA
Get access

Summary

Returning a rented car, the authors of this chapter tried to remember whose credit card had been imprinted when the car had been picked up. After some thought, the senior author reported a clear memory of giving the rental agent her card, and the junior author concurred that this had happened. Despite this clear shared memory, it was the junior author's card that had been imprinted.

People's memories and beliefs about events that may have happened to them can be true or false, and the consequences of being wrong are often more severe than those in the incident described above. Married couples argue about their divergent memories of past events, politicians are embarrassed when they misremember events that are a matter of popular record, and families can be destroyed when psychotherapy clients develop false memories of childhood abuse.

The central thesis of this chapter is that the answers people give to questions regarding the occurrence of events in their life are based on metacognitive decisional mechanisms. These mechanisms take two factors into account: (a) the information that is available about the event at the moment of the decision; and (b) the metacognitive beliefs that people hold. We maintain that the same decisional mechanisms and metacognitive beliefs that play a role when people have to answer questions about the occurrence of events that actually happened are also involved when people develop false beliefs and memories for events that did not happen to them.

Type
Chapter
Information
Applied Metacognition , pp. 121 - 145
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barnes, A. E., Dunlosky, J., Mazzoni, G., Narens, L., and Nelson, T. O. (1998). An integrative system of metamemory components involved in retrieval. In D. Gopher and A. Koriat (eds.), Attention and performance, cognitive regulation of performance: interaction of theory and application, Volume ⅠⅦ. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Barclay, C. R. (1996). Autobiographical remembering: narrative constraints on objectified selves. In D. C. Rubin (ed.), Remembering our past: studies in autobiographical memory, pp. 94–128. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Brown, A. L. (1975). The development of memory: knowing, knowing about knowing and knowing how to know. In W. Reese (ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, Volume 10. New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Chiesi, F., and Mazzoni, G. (1996). Alcune variabili che influenzano i falsi ricordi. Paper presented at the meeting of Associazione Italia Psicologia, Capri
Conway, M. A., Collins, A. F., Gathercole, S. E. and Anderson, S. J. (1996). Recollections of true and false autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 69–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261–288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, M. A., and Ross, M. (1984). Getting what you want by revising what you had. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 738–748CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornoldi, C. (1998). The impact of metacognitive reflection on cognitive control. In G. Mazzoni and T. Nelson (eds.), Metacognition and cognitive neuropsychology: monitoring and control processes, pp. 139–159. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Cornoldi, C., Gobbo, C., and Mazzoni, G. (1990). On metamemory–memory relationship: strategy availability and training. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 15, 46–58Google Scholar
Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, F., and Strack, J. (1998). Self-reflection and recognition: the role of metacognitive knowledge in the attribution of recollective experience. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 111–123Google Scholar
Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Memory and Cognition, 16, 309–313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardiner, J. M., and Java, R. I. (1993). Recognizing and remembering. In A. Collins, M. A. Conway, S. E. Gathercole, and P. E. Morris (eds.), Theories of memory, pp. 163–168. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Garry, M., Manning, C., Loftus, E. F., and Sherman, S. J. (1996). Imagination inflation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 208–214CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirt, E. R. (1990). Do I see what I expect? Evidence for an expectancy-guided retrieval model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 937–951CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, I. E., and Billings, F. J. (1998). Individual differences and the creation of false childhood memories. Memory, 6, 1–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, I. E., Husband, T. H., and Billings, F. J. (1995). False memories of childhood experiences. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, 181–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, I. E., and Kleinknecht, E. E. (1999). False childhood memories: research, theory and applications. In L. Williams and V. Banyard (eds.), Trauma and memory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications
Hyman, I. E., and Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, J. J., and Whitehouse, K. (1996). Remembering, knowing, and feeling of knowing. In L. M. Reder (ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition, pp. 287–307. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., and Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, C. M., and Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Memory attributions: remembering, knowing, and feeling of knowing. In L. M. Reder (ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition, pp. 287–307. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Koriat, A., and Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103, 490–517CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, September 1997, 72–77CrossRef
Loftus, E. F., Coan, J. A., and Pickrell, J. E. (1996). Manufacturing false memories with bits of reality. In L. M. Reder (ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition, pp. 195–220. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Malinowski, P. T., and Lynn, S. J. (1999). The plasticity of very early memory reports: social pressure, hypnotizability, compliance and interrogative suggestibility. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 47, 320–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzoni, G. (1999). When events become autobiographical: the role of metacognitive judgments. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Boulder, CO
Mazzoni, G., Chiesi, F., and Primi, C. (2000). Conoscenze autobiografiche dell'infanzia. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 27, 701–718Google Scholar
Mazzoni, , G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., and Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible autobiographical events: a little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 51–59Google Scholar
Mazzoni, G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., Seitz, A., and Lynn, S. J. (1999). Changing beliefs and memories through dream interpretation. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 125–1443.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzoni, G., and Vannucci, M. (1999). The provision of new information can change beliefs and memories about autobiographical events. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Boulder, CO
Metcalfe, J. (1993). Novelty monitoring, metacognition and control in a composite holographic associative recall model: implication for Korsakoff amnesia. Psychological Review, 100, 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, T. O., and Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: a theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Volume 26, pp. 125–173. New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Pezdek, K., Finger, K., and Hodge, D. (1997). Planting false childhood memories: the role of event plausibility. Psychological Science, 8, 437–441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L., and McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814Google Scholar
Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychological Review, 96, 341–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W. (1985). Developmental trends in metamemory–memory relationship: an integrative review. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley, G. E. McKinnon, and T. G. Waller (eds.), Cognition, metacognition and human performance, pp. 57–109. New York: Academic Press
Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., Kirsch, I., and Dugan, M. T. (2000, October). Rates of retrieval for true versus fabrication of false childhood events. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, Seattle
Snyder, M., and Uranowitz, S. W. (1978). Reconstructing the past: some cognitive consequences of person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 941–950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spanos, N. P. (1996). Multiple identities and false memories. Washington, DC: APA
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., Burgess, M. F., Samuels, C., and Blois, W. O. (1999). Creating false memories of infancy with hypnotic and nonhypnotic procedures. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 201–2183.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strack, F., and Bless, H. (1994). Memory for non-occurrence. Metacognitive and presuppositional strategies. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 203–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×