Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 56
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
September 2009
Print publication year:
2005
Online ISBN:
9780511489747

Book description

Infants' Sense of People focusses on infants during their first year of life, exploring how they begin to think about other people, their feelings, emotions and intentions, and how they become aware of these aspects of their own development. Drawing on a broad range of research and developmental theory, Maria Legerstee takes the view that infants have an innate sense of people at birth, which is activated through sympathetic emotions. She questions the idea that infants use physical parameters such as contingencies or motion to distinguish people from objects, and rejects the assumption that infants are mechanical creatures before they become psychological ones. She argues persuasively that before infants learn to speak, interactions with others are possible because infants have a primitive pre-linguistic 'theory of mind'. This accessible book provides a valuable synthesis of current thinking on early social and cognitive development and the origins of theory of mind.

Reviews

'The book provides a valuable synthesis of current theoretical approaches and research on the roots of social and cognitive development in general, with a focus on children's Theory of Mind.'

Pnina S. Klein - Bar-Ilan University, Israel

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

References
Adamson, L. B. and Bakeman, R. (1982). Affectivity and reference: Concepts, methods, and techniques in the study of communication development of 6– to 18-month-old infants. In Field, T. M. and Fogel, A. (eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Astington, J. W. (2001). The paradox of intention: Assessing children's metarepresentational understanding. In Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J., and Baldwin, D. A. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 85–103). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Bahrick, L. R. (1988) Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.
Bahrick, L. R., Moss, L., and Fadil, C. (1996). Development of visual self-recognition in infancy. Ecological Psychology, 8, 189–208.
Bahrick, L. R., and Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–973.
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6–8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.
Baillargeon, R.(1993). The object concept revisited: New directions in the investigation of infants' physical knowledge. In Granrud, C. (ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–315). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bakeman, R., and Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J. (1994). The mindreading engine: Evaluating evidence for modularity. Cahiers der Psychologie, 13, 553–560.
Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J(1996). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 133–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Social and ethical interpretations in mental development. New York: Macmillan.
Ball, A. W. (1973). The perception of causality in the infant. Presented at the Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.
Ball, A. W., and Tronick, E. (1971). Infant responses to impending collision: Optical and real. Science, 171, 818–820.
Bandura, A. (1962). Social cognitive theory of social referencing. In Feinman, S. (ed.), Social learning through imitation. University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE.
Banks, M. S., and Salapatek, P. (1983). Infant visual perception. In Haith, M. M. and Campos, J. J. (eds.), Infancy and developmental psychobiology (vol. II of P. H. Mussen [ed.], Handbook of Child Psychology), 4th edn. New York: Wiley.
Barna, J., and Legerstee, M. (2005). Nine and 12-month-old infants relate emotions to people's actions. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 53–67.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children “behaviorists”? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.
Baron-Cohen, S.(1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: Understanding attention in others. In Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Baron-Cohen, S.(1993). From attentional-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology. The development of theory of mind and its dysfunction. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 59–82). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baron-Cohen, S.(1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., and Walker, S. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes?British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.
Barrera, M. E., and Maurer, D. (1981). Recognition of mother's photographed face by the three-month-old infant. Child Development, 52, 714–716.
Barresi, J. (1984). Knowledge and representation: Transparent and opaque. Contemporary Psychology, 29, 160–161.
Barresi, J., and Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–154.
Barrett, M. D. (1989). Early language development. In Slater, A. and Bremner, J. G. (eds.), Infant development (pp. 211–241). London: Erlbaum.
Bartsch, K., and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baumwell, L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Borstein, M. H. (1997). Maternal verbal sensitivity and child language comprehension. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 247–258.
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., March, J. E., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
Beebe, B., Stern, D., and Jaffe, J. (1979). The kinesic rhythm of mother-infant interactions. In Siegman, A. W. & Feldstein, S. (eds.), Of speech and time: Temporal patterns in interpersonal contexts. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bellagamba, F. and Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12– and 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 277–282.
Berthenthal, B. I., and Bai, D. L. (1989). Infants' sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology, 25, 936–45.
Berkley, G. (1975). Philosophical works. Edited by Ayers, M. R.. London: Dent.
Bigelow, A. E. (1998). Infants' sensitivity to familiar imperfect contingencies in social interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 149–161.
Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Children's thinking. Cognitive development and individual differences. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson.
Bornstein, M. H., and Lamb, M. E. (1992). Development in infancy: An introduction (3rd edn, xiv, pp. 560). New York, NY, England: Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.
Bornstein, M. H., and Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 57, 251–274.
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1989a). Maternal responsiveness and cognitive development in children. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 49–61). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1989b). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.
Bornstein, M. H., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S.(1997). Maternal responsiveness and infant mental abilities: Specific predictive relations. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 283–296.
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. London: HMSO; New York: Columbia University.
Bowlby, J.(1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. I: Attachment. Pelican Books, Penguin.
Bowlby, J.(1979). On knowing what you are not supposed to know and feeling what you are not supposed to feel. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 24, 403–408.
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., and Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother–infant interaction. In Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp. 49–76). New York: Wiley.
Bremner, J. G. (1988). Infancy. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Bremner, J. G.(1998). From perception to action: The early development of knowledge. In Simion, F. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), The development of sensory, motor and cognitive capacities in early infancy: From perception to cognition (pp. 239–255). Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum.
Brentano, F. (1874/1973). Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Trans. A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrel, and L. L. McAlister. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Bretherton, I. and Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). A report on longitudinal evaluations of pre-school programs, Vol. 2, Is early intervention effective? Washington, DC: D. H. W. Publications No. (OHD) 74–25.
Bruner, J. S. (1973). Organization of early skilled action. Child Development, 44, 11.
Bruner, J. S.(1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255–287.
Bruner, J. S.(1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York: Norton.
Bruner, J. S.(1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J.(1999). The intentionality of referring. In Zelazo, P. and Astington, J. W. (eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 329–339). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bruner, J. S., and Koslowski, B. (1972). Visually preadapted constituents of manipulatory action. Perception, 1, 3–14.
Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., and Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother's face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.
Butterworth, G. (1990). On reconceptualizing sensorimotor development in dynamic systems theory. In Bloch, H. and Berthenthal, B. J. (eds.), Sensorimotor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 57–73). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Butterworth, G.(1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In Whiten, A. (ed.), National theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mind reading (pp. 223–232). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Butterworth, G.(1994). Theories of mind and the facts of embodiment. In Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development (pp. 115–132). Sussex, England: Erlbaum.
Butterworth, G.(1995). Factors in visual attention eliciting manual pointing in human infancy. In Raiblat, H. L. and Meyer, J. (eds.), Comparative approaches to cognitive science (pp. 329–338). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Butterworth, G., and Hicks, L. (1977). Visual proprioception and postural stability in infancy: A developmental study. Perception, 6, 255–262.
Butterworth, G., and Hopkins, B. (1988). Hand–mouth coordination in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 303–314.
Butterworth, G., and Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds have in common in space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55–72.
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., and Colonnesi, R. (2004), The role of declarative pointing in developing a Theory of Mind. Infancy, 5, 291–308.
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., and Milone, A. (1997). Brief report: A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725.
Campbell, S. B., Cohn, J., and Myers, T. (1995). Depression in first-time mothers: Mother–infant interaction and depression chronicity. Developmental Psychology, 31, 349–357.
Campos, J., and Sternberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. 273–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Caron, A., Carfon, R., Mustelin, C., and Roberts, J. (1992). Infant responding to aberrant social stimuli. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 335 (Special ICIS Conference Issue).
Carpenter, M., Nagell, L., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63 (4, serial no. 255).
Chada, S. W. (1996). A comparison of mother–infant, father–infant and stranger–infant affect attunement: Characteristics and consequences of discrete parenting roles in infant affective development. Dissertation Abstracts International, 52 (12-B), 7060.
Chapman, M. (1992). Equilibration and the dialectic of organization. In Beiling, H. and Putfall, P. (eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities (pp. 39–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., and Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.
Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., and Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.
Chi, M. T. H. (1988). Children's lack of access and knowledge reorganization: An example from the concept of animacy. In Weinert, F. E. and Perlmuter, M. (eds.), Memory development: Universal changes and individual differences (pp. 169–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Churchland, P. M. (1991). Folk-psychology and the explanation of human behavior. In Greenwood, J. D. (ed.), The future of folk psychology: Intentionality and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cicchetti, D., and Schneider-Rosen, K. (1984). Toward a transactional model of childhood depression. New Directions for Child Development, 26, 5–27.
Cohn, J. F., and Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Specificity of infants' response to mothers' affective behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 242–248.
Corkum, V., and Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development (pp. 61–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Corkum, V., and Moore, C.(1998). The origin of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 28–38.
Coster, W. J., Gersten, M. S., Beegly, M., and Cicchetti, D. (1989). Communicative functioning in maltreated toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 25, 1020–1029.
Crockenberg, S. (1983). Early mother and infant antecedents of Bayley Scale performance at 21 months. Developmental Psychology, 19, 727–730.
Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., and Brockbank, M. (1999). Social attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–294.
Davis, K. (1947). Final note on a case of extreme isolation. American Journal of Sociology, 52, 432–437.
DeCasper, A. J., and Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.
DeCasper, A. J., and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother's voice. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
DeCasper, A. J., Lecanuet, J. P., Busnell, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., and Maugeais, R. (1994). Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–150.
DeCharmes, R. (1968). Personal Causation: The Internal Affective Determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Descartes, R. (1641/1985). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vols. 1 and 2). Trans. J. Cottingham et al. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Desrochers, S., Morrissette, P., and Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dodd, B. (1979). Lip reading in infants: Attention to speech present in- and out-of-synchrony. Cognitive psychology, 11, 478–484.
Dondi, M., Simion, F., and Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant?Developmental Psychology, 35, 418–426.
Dunham, P., and Dunham, F. (1990). Effects of mother–infant social interactions on infants' subsequent contingency task performance. Child Development, 61, 785–793.
Dunham, P., Dunham, F., Hurshman, A., and Alexander, T. (1989). Social contingency effects on subsequent perceptual-cognitive tasks in young infants. Child Development, 60, 1486–1496.
Emde, R. N. (1989). Early emotional development: New modes of thinking for research and intervention. In Warhol, J. G. (ed.), New perspectives in early emotional development (pp. 29–45). Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute.
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Juscyk, P., and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 171, 303–306.
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. New York: Prentice-Hall.
Ellsworth, C. P., Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1993). Social competence and person–object differentiation: An analysis of the still-face effect. Developmental Psychology, 39, 63–73.
Fagan, J. F. (1972). Infants' recognition memory for faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 453–476.
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470.
Feinman, S., and Lewis, M. (1983). Social referencing at 10-months. A second order effect on infants' responses to strangers. Child Development, 54, 878–887.
Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behaviour and Development, 7, 527–532.
Field, T.(1990). Neonatal stress and coping in intensive care. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11, 57–65.
Field, T.(1992). Psychobiological attunement in close relationships. In Lerner, R., Featherman, D. L., and Perlmutter, M. (eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 2–22). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Field, T.(1994). The effects of mother's physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (23).
Field, T.(1995). Infant of depressed mothers (Presidential address). Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 1–13.
Field, T., Cohen, D., Garcia, , and Greenberg, R. (1985). Mother–stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Annual Progress in Child Psychiatry and Child Development, 3–10.
Field, T., Guy, L., and Umbel, V. (1985). Infants' responses to mothers' imitative behaviors. Infant Mental Health Journal, 6, 40–44.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., Berry, D., Schanberg, S., Zimmerman, E. A., and Kuhn, C. (1998). Infants of depressed mothers show “depressed” behavior even with nondepressed adults. Child Development, 59, 1569–1579.
Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions of neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.
Finkelstein, N. W., and Ramey, C. T. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., and Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organize development. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 81–127.
Fisher, L., Ames, E. W., Chisholm, K., and Savoie, L. (1997). Problems reported by parents of Romanians orphans adopted to British Columbia. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 67–82.
Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of the children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. In Astington, J. W. and Harris, P. L. (eds.), Developing theories of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Flavell, J. H.(1999). Cognitive development: Children's knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21–45.
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., and Moses, L. J. (1990). Young children's understanding of false beliefs versus value beliefs. Child Development, 61, 915–928.
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–68.
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. A.(1992). A theory of the child's theory of mind. Cognition, 44, 283–296.
Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother–infant face-to-face interaction. In Schaffer, H. Rudolph (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 5–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fogel, A.(1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self and culture. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Fogel, A.(2001). The history (and future) of infancy. Blackwell handbook of infant development. Handbooks of developmental psychology (pp. 726–757). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.
Fogel, A., and Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of nine to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interaction with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.
Fogel, A., and Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761.
Franco, F., and Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23, 307–336.
Freeman, W. J. (2000). Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In Granic, I. and Lewis, M. (eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 209–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psychoanalysis (rev. edn, Strachey). New York: Norton (original work published 1940).
Freud, S.(1961). The ego and the id. New York: W. W. Norton and Co.
Frye, D. (1981). Developmental changes in strategies of social interaction. In Lamb, M. and Sherrod, L. (eds.), Infant social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gallagher, S. (1996). The moral significance of primitive self-consciousness: The response to Bermúdez. Ethics, 107, 129–140.
Gallup, G. G. (1982). Self-awareness and the emergence of mind in primates. American Journal of Primatology, 2, 237–248.
Gekoski, M. J., and Fagen, J. W. (1984). Noncontingent stimulation, stimulus familiarization, and subsequent learning in young infants. Child Development, 55, 2226–2233.
Gelman, R., and Spelke, E. (1981). The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In Flavell, J. H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognition development: Frontiers and possible futures. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gelman, S. A., and Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in 2-year-old children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 796–804.
Gelman, S. A., Durgin, F., and Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by notion alone. In Sperber, D. and Premack, D. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 150–184). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Gelman, S. A., and Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.
Gergely, G. (2001). Is early differentiation of human behavior a precursor to the 1-year-old's understanding of intentional action? Comment on Legerstee, Barna, and DiAdamo (2000). Developmental Psychology, 37, 579–582.
Gergely, G.(2002). The development of understanding self and agency. In Goswamia, Usha (ed.), Blackwell's Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Gergely, G., and Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: The development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 77, 1181–1212.
Gergely, G., and Watson, J.(1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101–136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibry, G., and Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.
Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. East Norwalk, US: Appleton Century Crofts.
Gibson, E. J.(1993). Ontogenesis of the perceived self. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 25–42). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gibson, E. J.(1995). Are we automata? In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology (pp. 13–15). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Glick, J. (1978). Cognition and social cognition: an introduction. In Glick, J. L. and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.), The development of social understanding, (pp. 1–9). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Glick, J., and Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (eds.) (1978). The development of social understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Goldberg, S., Lojkasek, M., Gartner, G., and Corter, C. (1989). Maternal responsiveness and social development in pre-term infants. In Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Maternal responsiveness: Characteristics and consequences (pp. 89–103). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Goldfarb, W. (1943). Infant rearing and problem behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 13, 249–265.
Goldsmith, D. F., and Rogoff, B. (1997). Mothers' and toddlers' coordinated joint focus of attention: Variations with maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 33, 113–119.
Gopnik, A. (1995). How to understand beliefs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 398–400.
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gopnik, A., and Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child's theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language, 7, 145–171.
Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. Y. K. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics, 56, 544–549.
Gotlieb, S. J. (1991). Visual memory in neonates. Dissertation Abstracts International, 51 (10-B), 5049–5050.
Granic, I. (2000) Emotion, development, and self-organization: dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), The self-organization of parent–child relations: Beyond bidirectional models (pp. 267–297). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Grant, K. W., Ardell, L. H., Kuhl, P. K., and Sparks, D. W. (1986). The transmission of prosodic information via an electrotactile speedreading aid. Ear and Hearing, 7, 243–251.
Hains, S. M., and Muir, D. W. (1996). Effects of stimulus contingency in infant–adult interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 19, 49–61.
Haith, M. M. (1966). The response of the human newborn to visual movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Child Psychology, 3, 243–253.
Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., and Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face scanning in early infancy. Science, 198, 853–855.
Harding, C. G. (1982). Development of the intention to communicate. Human Development, 25, 140–151.
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673–685.
Harlow, H. F.(1961). The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour (Vol. I). London: Methuen.
Harlow, H. F. and Harlow, M. K. (1965). The affectional systems. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F., and Stollnitz, F. (eds.), Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol. II, pp. 287–334). New York: Academic Press.
Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1959). Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science, 130, 421–432.
Harris, P. L. (1989). Children and emotion: The development of psychological understanding. Oxford, England: Basil Blackwell.
Harris, P. L.(1992). From simulation to folk psychology: The case for development. Mind Language, 7, 120–144.
Haviland, J. M., and Lewica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants' responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.
Heyes, C. M. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, bbsonline.org.
Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition: A theory of autism. In Dawson, G. (ed.), Autism, Nature, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 48–). New York, Guilford.
Hobson, R. P.(1989). On sharing experiences. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 197–203.
Hobson, R. P.(1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163–181.
Hobson, R. P.(1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove: Erlbaum.
Hobson, R. P.(1998). The intersubjective foundations of thought. In Braten, S. (ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: Studies in emotion and social interaction (2nd series, pp. 283–296). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hobson, R. P.(2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hoffman, M. (1981). Perspectives on the difference between understanding people and understanding things: the role of affect. In Flavell, H. and Ross, L. (eds.), Social cognitive development: Frontiers and possible futures, pp. 67–81. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hofsten, C. (1980). Predictive reaching for moving objects by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 369–382.
Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., and Driver, J. (1998). Adults' eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9, 131–134.
Hornick, R., Risenhoover, N., and Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.
Hsu, H. C., and Fogel, A., (2003). Stability and transitions in mother–infant face-to-face communication during the first 6 months: A micro-historical approach. Developmental Psychology, 39, 1061–1082.
Hsu, H. C., Fogel, A., and Messinger, D. S. (2001). Infant non-distress vocalization during mother–infant face-to-face interaction: Factors associated with quantitative and qualitative differences. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 107–128.
Hume, D. (1739/1888). A treatise of human nature. Ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A.. Oxford: Clarendon.
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. East Norwalk: Appleton Century Crofts.
Izard, C. E.(1978). Emotions as motivations: An evolutionary-developmental perspective. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 26, 163–200.
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., and Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66, 2001.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H. D., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.
Johnson, M. H., and Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Johnson, S. C., Booth, A., and O'Hearn, L. (2001). Inferring the goals of a non-human agent. Cognitive Development, 16, 637–656.
Johnson, S., Slaughter, V., and Carey, S. (1998). Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science, 1, 233–238.
Juscyk, P. W., Kennedy, L. J., and Juscyk, A. (1995). Young infants' retention of information about syllables. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 24–41.
Kagan, J. (1976). Resilience and continuity in psychological development. In Clarke, A. M. and Clarke, A. D. B. (eds.), Early experience: Myth and evidence (pp. 97–121). New York: The Free Press.
Kalins, I. V., and Bruner, J. S. (1973). The coordination of visual observation and instrumental behavior in early infancy. Perception, 2, 307–314.
Kant, I. (1781/1996). Critique of pure reason. Trans. W. S. Pluhar. Indianapolis: Hackett.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Karmiloff-Smith, A.(1998). Is atypical development necessarily a window on the normal mind/brain? The case of Williams Syndrome. Developmental-Science, 1, 273–277.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.
Kaye, K. (1982). Self-image formation in adopted children: The environment within. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 13, 175–181.
Kaye, K., and Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 454–464.
Kaye, K., and Markus, J. (1981). Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda. Developmental Psychology, 3, 258–265.
Keil, C. K., Smith, W. C., Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1998). Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: Implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge. Cognition, 65, 103–135.
Kenny, J. (1988), The self. Marquette: Marquette University Press.
Killen, M., and Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Imitation of action with objects: The role of social meaning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 138, 219–229.
Koluchova, J. (1972). Severe deprivation in twins: A case study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 13, 107–114.
Kuchuk, A., Vibbert, N. M., and Bornstein, M. H. (1986). The perception of smiling and its experiential correlates in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 57, 1054–1061.
Kuhl, P., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.
LaBarbera, J. D., Izard, C. E., Vietze, P., and Parisi, S. A. (1976). Four- and six-month-old infants' visual responses to joy, anger, and neutral expressions. Child Development, 47, 535–538.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Millar-Loncar, C. L., and Swank, P. R. (1998). The relation of change in maternal interactive styles to the developing social competence in full-term and pre-term children. Child Development, 69, 105–123.
Legerstee, M. (1990). Infants use multimodal stimulation to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 345–356.
Legerstee, M.(1991a). Changes in the quality of infant sounds as a function of social and nonsocial stimulation. First Language, 11, 327–343.
Legerstee, M.(1991b). The role of people and objects in early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.
Legerstee, M.(1992). A review of the animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Implications for models of social and cognitive knowing. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 59–67.
Legerstee, M.(1994a). Patterns of 4-month-old infant responses to hidden silent and sounding people and objects. Early Development and Parenting, 3, 71–80.
Legerstee, M.(1994b). The role of familiarity and sound in the development of person and object permanence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 455–468.
Legerstee, M.(1997a). Changes in social-conceptual development: domain specific structures, self-organization and indeterminism. In Fogel, A., Lyra, M. C. D. P., and Valsiner, J. (eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in development and social processes (pp. 245–260). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Legerstee, M.(1997b). Contingency effects of people and objects on subsequent cognitive functioning in three-month-old infants. Social Development, 6, 307–321.
Legerstee, M.(1998). Mental and bodily awareness in infancy: Consciousness of self-existence. Journal of Consciousness, 5, 627–644.
Legerstee, M.(2001a). Domain specificity and the epistemic triangle in the development of animism in infancy. In Lacerda, F., Hofsten, C., and Heinemann, M. (eds.), Emerging cognitive abilities in early infancy (pp. 193–212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Legerstee, M.(2001b). Six-month-old infants rely on explanatory inference when relating communication to people and manipulatory actions to inanimate objects: Reply to Gergely. Developmental Psychology, 5, 583–586.
Legerstee, M., Anderson, D., and Schaffer, A. (1998). Five- and eight-month-old infants recognize their faces and voices as familiar and social stimuli. Child Development, 69, 37–50.
Legerstee, M., and Barillas, Y. (2003). Sharing attention and pointing to objects at 12 months: Is the intentional stance implied?Cognitive Development, 18, 91–110.
Legerstee, M., Barna, J., and DiAdamo, C. (2000). Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: Understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology, 3, 627–634.
Legerstee, M., and Bowman, T. (1989). The development responses to people and a toy in infants with Down syndrome. Infant Behaviour and Development, 12, 462–473.
Legerstee, M., Bowman, T., and Fels, S. (1992). People and objects affect the quality of vocalizations in infants with Down syndrome. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 149–156.
Legerstee, M., Corter, C., and Kienapple, K. (1990). Hand, arm and facial actions of young infants to a social and nonsocial stimulus. Child Development, 61, 774–784.
Legerstee, M., Fahy, L., Blake, J., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (May, 2004). Maternal Factors Contributing to Toddlers' Early Development of Internal State Language: A Longitudinal Study. Poster presented at 14th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), Chicago, USA.
Legerstee, M., and Feider, H. (1986). The acquisition of person pronouns in French-speaking children. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 629–639.
Legerstee, M., Fisher, T., and Markova, G. (2005). The development of attention during dyadic and triadic interactions: The role of affect attunement. Paper presented at 35th Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., and Markova, G. (2005). Variation in imitation in 10-month-old infants: Awareness of intentional action. Paper presented at 35th annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Vancouver, Canada, June, 2005.
Legerstee, M., Pasic, N., Barillas, Y., and Fahy, L. (2004). Social emotional development: The basis for mentalism. In Gallagher, S., Watson, S., LeBrun,, P. and Romanski, P.. (eds.) Ipseity and alterity: Interdisciplinary approaches to intersubjectivity (pp. 33–46). Rouen, France: Publications de l'Université de Rouen.
Legerstee, M., Pomerlau, A., Malacuit, G., and Feider, H. (1987). The development of infants' responses to people and a doll: Implications for research in communication. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 81–95.
Legerstee, M., Beek, Y., and Varghese, M. (2002). Effects of maintaining and redirecting infant attention on the production of referential communication in infants with and without Down syndrome. Journal of Child Language, 29, 23–48.
Legerstee, M., and Varghese, J. (2001). The role of affect mirroring on social expectancies in three-month-old infants. Child Development, 72, 1301–1313.
Legerstee, M., and Weintraub, J. (1997). The integration of person and object attention in infants with and without Down Syndrome. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 71–82.
Leslie, A. M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception, 13, 287–305.
Leung, E. H., and Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215–220.
Lewis, M. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self., New York, Plenum.
Lewis, M. D. (1997). Personality self-organization: Cascading constraints on cognition-emotion interaction. In Lyra, M. (ed.), et-al. Fogel, A. (ed.), Dynamics and indeterminism in developmental and social processes (pp. 193–216). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Lewis, M. D.(2000). Emotional self-organization at three time scales, The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy (pp. 37–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lewis, M. D., and Granic, I. (2000). The dynamics of emotion-related behaviors in infancy. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Leyendecker, B., Lamb, M. E., Schölmerich, A., and Fricke, D. M. (1997). Contexts as moderators of observed interactions: A study of Costa Rican mothers and infants from differing socioeconomic backgrounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 15–34.
Lillard, J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Young children's preference for mental state versus behavioral descriptions of human actions. Child Development, 61, 731–741.
Locke, A. (1980). The Guided Reinvention of Language. London: Academic Press.
Locke, J. (1710/1975). An essay concerning human understanding (ed. with a foreword by P. H. Nidditsch). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Lollis, S., and Kuczynski, L. (1997). Beyond one hand clapping: Seeing bidirectionality in parent–child relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 441–461.
Loveland, K. A. (1986). Discovering the affordances of a reflecting surface. Developmental Review, 6, 1–24.
Lukesch, H., and Lukesch, M. (1976). Fragebogen zur Messung von Einstellungen zu Schwangerschaft, Sexualität und Geburt (SSG). Göttingen: Hogrefe.
MacKain, K., Studder-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., and Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 218, 1138–1141.
Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., and Bergman, A. (1975). Stages in the infant's separation from the mother. In Handel, G. and Whitchurch, G. G. (eds.), The psychosocial interior of the family (4th edn, pp. 419–448). Hawthorne, US: Aldine de Gruyter.
Malatesta, C. Z., and Izard, C. E. (1984). The ontogenesis of human social signals: From biological imperative to symbol utilization. In Fox, N. A. and Davidson, R. J. (eds.), The Psychobiology of Affective Development (pp. 161–206). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mandler, J. M. (1992). How to build a baby: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.
Maratos, O. (1973). The origin and development of imitation in the first six months of life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Geneva.
Markova, G. (Winter, 2004). PhD student participant in Graduate seminar, “Development of Affect, Consciousness and Social Cognition,” (M. Legerstee). York University.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M. (in press). Contingency, imitation or affect sharing? Foundations of infant's social awareness, Developmental Psychology.
Markova, G., and Legerstee, M.(2004). Foundations of infant's social awareness. Paper presented at the Conference on Developmental relations between Executive Functioning, Social Understanding, and Social Interaction, September, Vancouver, BC).
Markus, J., Mundy, P., Morales, M., Delgado, C. E. F., and Yale, M. (2000). Individual differences in infant skills as predictors of child–caregiver joint attention and language. Social Development, 9, 302–315.
Maurer, D., and Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development, 47, 523–527.
McCall, R., and Kagan, J. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3 (3, pt. 1).
McGurk, H. and McDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: Mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Sciences, 42, 637–648.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, 1715–1726.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Clark-Carter, D., Gupta, M. D., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M.(2003). Pathways to understanding mind: Construct validity and predictive validity of maternal mind-mindedness. Child Development, 74, 1194–1211.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1985). Immediate and deferred imitation in fourteen- and twenty-four-month-old infants. Child Development, 56, 62–72.
Meltzoff, A. N.(1988). Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59, 217–225.
Meltzoff, A. N.(1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modelling, and self practice in infancy. In Cicchetti, D. and Beeghly, M. (eds.), The self in transition: Infancy to childhood (pp. 139–164). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Meltzoff, A. N.(1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Reenactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Borton, R. (1979). Intermodal matching of human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In Malle, B. F. and Bertram, L. J. (eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–195). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism (pp. 335–365). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures of human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1983). The origins of imitation in infancy: Paradigm, phenomena, and theories. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. (eds.), Advances in infancy research, 2. Norwood: NJ, Ablex.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–192.
Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K.(1999). Persons and representation: Why infant imitation is important for theories of human development. In Nadel, J. and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 9–35). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1942/1963). The structure of behavior. Trans. Fisher, A. L.. USA: Beacon Press.
Messer, D. (1997). Referential communication: Making sense of the social and physical worlds. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 291–309). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Messer, S. B., Kagan, J., and McCall, R. B. (1970). Fixation time and tempo of play in infants. Developmental Psychology, 3, 406.
Millar, W. S. (1972). A study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcements in early infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 37, 1–44.
Moerk, E. L. (1989). The fuzzy set called “imitation.” In Speidel, G. E. and Nelson, K. E. (eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 277–303). New York: Springer.
Molina, M., Van de Walle, G. A., Condry, K., and Spelke, E. S. (in press). The animate/inanimate distinction in infancy: Developing sensitivity to constraints on human actions. Journal of Cognition and Development.
Montague, D. P. F., and Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2001). Peekaboo: A new look at infants' perception of emotion expression. Developmental Psychology, 37, 826–838.
Moore, C. (1996). Theories of mind in infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 19–40. In C. Moore, and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
Moore, C., and Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review, 14, 349–372.
Moore, C., and D' Entremont, B. (2001). Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 109–129.
Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., and Anderson, M. (1992, September). IQ-independent person perception: Evidence from developmental psychopathology. Paper presented at the meeting of the 5th European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Seville, Spain.
Morrissette, P., Ricard, M., and Decarie, T. G. (1995). Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: A longitudinal study of comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 163–175.
Moses, L. J. (1993). Young children's understanding of belief constraints on intention. Cognitive Development, 8, 1–25.
Moses, L. J., Baldwin, D. A., Rosicky, J. G., and Tidball, G. (2001). Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months. Child Development, 72, 718–735.
Moses, L. J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). Inferring false beliefs from actions and reactions. Child Development, 61, 929–945.
Muir, D., Cao, Y., and Entremont, B. (1994, June). Infant social perception revealed during adult face-to-face interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 86.
Muir, D., and Hains, S. (1999). Young infants' perception of adult intentionality: Adult contingency and eye direction. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 155–187). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Munakata, Y. (2000). Challenges to the violation-of-expectation paradigm: Throwing the conceptual baby out with the perceptual processing bathwater?Infancy, 1, 471–477.
Mundy, P., and Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.
Murphy, C. M., and Messer, D. J. (1977). Mothers, infants, and pointing: A study of gesture. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 325–354). London: Academic Press.
Murray, L., Fior-Cowley, A., Hooper, R., and Cooper, P. (1996). The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcomes. Child Development, 67, 2512–2526.
Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. (eds.), Social perception in infants. New Jersey: Ablex.
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., and Reserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164–173.
Nakano, S. and Kanaya, Y. (1993). The effects of mothers' teasing: Do Japanese infants read their mothers' play intention in teasing?Early Development and Parenting, 2, 7–17.
Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Neisser, U.(1995). Criteria for an ecological self. In Rochat, P. (eds.), The Self in Infancy: Theory and Research (pp. 17–34). Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/ Elsevier Science Publishers.
Nelson, L. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expression in the first two years of life: Mechanisms of development. Child Development, 58, 889–909.
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers' attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 557–568.
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Grolnick, W. S. (1999). Maternal responses to infant affect: Stability and prediction. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 103–117.
Nishida, T. (1986). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.), Primate Societies (pp. 462–471).
Papousek, M., Papousek, H., and Symmes, D. (1991). The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 415–440.
Pascual-Leone, J., and Johnson, J. (1998). A dialectical constructivist view of representation: The role of mental attention, executives, and symbols. In Sigel, I. E. (ed.), The development of representational thought: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 169–200). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Perry, J. C., and Stern, D. N. (1976). Gaze duration frequency distributions during mother–infant interaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 129, 45–55.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Phillips, A. T., Wellman, H. M., and Spelke, E. (2002). Infant's ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action. Cognition, 85, 53–78.
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., and Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicaps. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 375–383.
Piaget, J. (1929). The child's conception of the world. Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Piaget, J.(1952). Play, dreams and imitation. New York: W. W. Norton and Co. Inc.
Piaget, J.(1954). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.
Piaget, J.(1981). Intelligence and affectivity: Their relationship during child development. Oxford: Annual Reviews.
Poulin-Dubois, D. (1999). Infants' distinction between animate and inanimate objects: The origins of naïve psychology. In Rochat, P. (ed.), Early social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Povinelli, D. J., and Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, (3, Serial No. 247).
Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.
Premack, D.(1991). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. In Frye, D. and Moore, C. (eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding (pp. 39–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rader, N., and Stern, J. D. (1982). Visually elicited reaching in neonates. Child Development, 53, 1004–1007.
Rakison, D. H., and Poulin-Dubois, D. (2000). Developmental origin of the animate/inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 209–226.
Ramey, C. T., and Finkelstein, N. W. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.
Raver, C. C., and Leadbeater, B. J. (1995). Factors influencing joint attention between socioeconomically disadvantaged adolescent mothers and their infants. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origin and role in development (pp. 251–271). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others' expectations: Teaching and mucking about in the first year. In Whitten, A. (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.
Reddy, V.(1999). Prelinguistic communication. In Barrett, M. (ed.), The development of language. Studies in developmental psychology (pp. 25–50). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Reddy, V.(2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self–other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 397–402.
Reddy, V., Hay, D., Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1997). Communication in infancy: Mutual regulation of affect and attention. In Bremner, G., Slater, A., and Butterworth, G. (eds.), Infant development: Recent advances (pp. 247–273). Hove: Psychology Press.
Reed, E. S. (1995). Becoming a self. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers.
Repacholi, B. M. (1998). Infants' use of attentional cues to identify the referent of another person's emotional expression. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1017–1025.
Repacholi, B. M., and Gopnik, A. (1997). Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14– and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.
Rheingold, H. L. (1961). The effect of environmental stimulation upon social and exploratory behaviour in the human infant. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 143–171). Oxford, England: Wiley.
Ribble, M. (1944). Infant experience in relation to personality development. In Hunt, M. V. (ed.), Personality and the Behavior Disorders (pp. 621–651). Ronald Press.
Rogers, S. J., and Pennington, B. F. (1991). A theoretical approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162.
Rothbart, M. K. (1981). IBQ. The Infant Behavior Questionnaire: Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.
Rovee-Collier, C. K., and Fagan, J. W. (1981). The retrieval of memory in early infancy. Advances in Infancy Research, 1, 225–254.
Ruddy, M. G., and Bornstein, M. H. (1982). Cognitive correlates of infant attention and maternal stimulation over the first year of life. Child Development, 53, 183–188.
Rutter, M. (1971). Parent–child separation: Psychological effects on the children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 233–260.
Rutter, M.(1972). Maternal deprivation reassessed. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Salapatek, P., and Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155–167.
Saxon, T. F., Frick, J. E., and Colombo, J. (1997). A longitudinal study of maternal interactional styles and infant visual attention. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 43, 48–66.
Scaife, M., and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265–266.
Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The Growth of Sociability. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.
Schaffer, H. R.(1984). The child's entry into a social world. London: Academic Press.
Schaffer, H. R., Collins, G. M., and Parsons, G. (1977). Vocal interchange and visual regard in verbal and preverbal children. In Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp. 291–324). London: Academic Press.
Schmuckler, M. (1981). Visual habituation programme. Ontario: University of Toronto.
Schore, A. N. (2000). Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (eds.), Neurobiological perspectives: The self-organization of the right brain and the neurobiology of emotional development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Schultz, S., and Wellman, H. M. (1997). Explaining human movements and actions: Children's understanding of the limits of psychological explanation. Cognition, 62, 291–324.
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A. J., Resnick, S., and Riordan, K. (1996). Attachment, maternal sensitivity, and infant temperament during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 32, 12–25.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman.
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Concurrent operants. American Psychologist, 3, 359.
Smith, P. B., and Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity and patterns of infant–mother attachment. Child Development, 59, 1097–1101.
Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Child Language, 4, 1–22.
Soken, N. H., and Pick, A. D. (1992). Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 63, 787–795.
Spelke, E. S. (1981). The infant's acquisition of knowledge of bimodally specified events. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 179–299.
Spelke, E. S.(1985). Preferential-looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy. In Gotlieb, G. and Krasnegor, N. A. (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life: A methodological overview. Westport, US: Ablex Publishing.
Spelke, E. S.(1988). Where perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, A. (ed.), Perceptual development in infancy. The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (vol. 20, pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., and Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–632.
Spelke, E. S., and Cortelyou, A. (1981). Perceptual aspects of social knowing: Looking and listening in infancy. In Lamb, M. E. and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., and Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants, knowledge of object motion and human action. In Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (eds.), Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate (pp. 44–78). Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. J. (1995). Causal cognition: A multi-disciplinary debate. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Spitz, R. (1963). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of Child, 1, 53–74.
Stankov, L. (1983). Attention and intelligence. Educational Psychology, 75, 471–490.
Stern, D. N. (1977). The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Stern, D. N.(1984). Affect attunement. In Call, J. D., Galenson, E., and Tyson, R. T. (eds.), Frontiers of infant psychiatry (Vol. II, pp. 3–14). New York: Basic Books.
Stern, D. N.(1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.
Stern, D. N.(1995). Self/other distinction in the domain of intimate socio-affective interaction: Some considerations. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in psychology. Amsterdam: North Holland-Elsevier.
Sullivan, J. W., and Horowitz, F. D. (1983). Infant intermodal perception and maternal multimodal stimulation: Implications for language development. In Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. K. (eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Summerfeld, Q. (1979). Use of visual information for phonetic perception. Phonetica, 36, 314–331.
Szajnberg, N. M., Skrinjaric, J., and Moore, A. (1989). Affect attunement, attachment, temperament and zygosity: A twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 249–253.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). An analysis of discourse ability and internal state lexicons in a longitudinal study of autistic children. Paper presented at Biennial Meeting of Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738–751.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., and Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.
Tarabulsy, G. M., Tessier, R., and Kappas, A. (1996). Contingency detection and the contingent organization of behavior in interactions: Implications for socioemotional development in infancy. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 25–41.
Teo, T. (1997). Developmental psychology and the relevance of a critical metatheoretical reflection. Human Development, 40, 195–210.
Tizard, B., and Rees, J. (1974). A comparison of the effects of adoption, restoration, to the natural mother, and continued institutionalization of the cognitive development of four-year-old children. Child Development, 45, 92–99.
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. and Dunham, P. (eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M.(1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. M. and Galef, B. G. (eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 319–346). New York: Academic Press.
Tomasello, M.(1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello, M.(2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 37–40.
Tomasello, M.(January, 2003), personal communication.
Tomasello, M., and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. London: Oxford University Press.
Tomasello, M., and Camaioni, L. (1997). A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Human Development, 40, 7–24.
Tomasello, M., and Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.
Trehub, S. (1976). The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 47, 466–472.
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication (pp. 321–347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C.(1991). The self born in intersubjectivity: The psychology of an infant communicating. In Neisser, U. (ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen, C., and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the second year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). New York: Academic Press.
Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.
Tronick, E. Z.(2002). A model of infant mood states: Long lasting organizing affective states and emotional representational processes without language or symbols. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 12, 73–99.
Tronick, E. Z.(2003). Things still to be done on the still-face effect. Infancy, 4, 475–482.
Tronick, E. Z.(2004). Selective coherence and dyadic states of consciousness. In Nadel, J. and Muir, D. (eds.), Developmental studies of emotions (pp. 293–315). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Valsiner, J. (1997). Culture and the development of children's action. New York: Wiley.
Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers with irritable infants. Child Development, 65, 1457–1477.
Vecera, S. P., and Johnson, M. H. (1995). Gaze detection and the cortical processing of faces: Evidence from infants and adults. Visual Cognition, 2, 59–87.
Vedeler, D. (1994). Infant intentionality as object-directedness: A method for observation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 35, 343–366.
Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., and Schoef, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, attachment, and competence in five year olds. Child Development, 67, 2493–2511.
Hofsten, C. (1982). Eye–hand coordination in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 18, 450–461.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (Edited and translated by Haufmann, Eugenia and Vakar, Gertrude.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky, L. S.(1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. In Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Criner, S., and Souberman, E. (eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Walden, T. A., and Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230–1240.
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 437–456.
Watson, J. B. (1928). Psychological care of infant and child. New York: Norton.
Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “the game.”Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.
Watson, J. S.(1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In Thoman, E. B. (ed.), The origins of the infant's social responsiveness (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Watson, J. S.(1985). Contingency perception in early social development. In Field, T. M. and Fox, N. A. (eds.), Social perception (pp. 157–176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.
Watson, J. S., and Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–227.
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Wellman, H. M.(1993). Early understanding of mind: The normal case. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 10–39). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Wellman, H. M., and Banerjee, M. (1991). Mind and emotion: Children's understanding of the emotional consequences of beliefs and desires. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 191–224.
Wellman, H. M., and Estes, D. (1986). Early understanding of mental entities: A reexamination of childhood realism. Child Development, 57, 910–923.
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion. Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–149.
Wellman, H. M., and Woolley, J. P. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.
Werker, J. F., and McLeod, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230–246.
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Wolff, P. H. (1966). The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. Psychological Issue, 5 (Monograph 17).
Wolff, P. H.(1987). The development of behavioral states and the expression of emotions in early infancy: New proposals for investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.
Yonas, A., Petterson, L, and Lockman, J. J. (1979). Young infants' sensitivity to optical information for collision. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 33, 268–276.
Zeedyk, S. M. (1996). Developmental accounts of intentionality: Toward integration. Developmental Review, 16, 416–461.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.