Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 87, Issue 3, April 2003, Pages B87-B95
Cognition

Brief article
The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00234-2Get rights and content

Abstract

In the domain of numbers the existence of spatial components in the representation of numerical magnitude has been convincingly demonstrated by an association between number magnitude and response preference with faster left- than right-hand responses for small numbers and faster right- than left-hand responses for large numbers (Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993) The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396). Because numbers convey not only real or integer meaning but also ordinal meaning, the question of whether non-numerical ordinal information is spatially coded naturally follows. While previous research failed to show an association between ordinal position and spatial response preference, we present two experiments involving months (Experiment 1) and letters (Experiment 2) in which spatial coding is demonstrated. Furthermore, the response-side effect was obtained with two different stimulus-response mappings. The association occurred both when ordinal information was relevant and when it was irrelevant to the task, showing that the spatial component of the ordinal representation can be automatically activated.

Introduction

Although considerable advances have been made in the understanding of the internal representation and processing of ordinal information (see Leth-Steensen & Marley, 2000, for a recent review), one aspect of it, the association between order and space, has barely been investigated. This is surprising because numbers, which convey ordinal meaning (hierarchically implied by their real and integer meaning; Gallistel & Gelman, 2000), are generally accepted to be spatially coded.

An explicit indication of an association between numbers and space was first demonstrated by a response-side effect in number comparison (Dehaene et al., 1990, Hinrichs et al., 1981). When participants had to indicate whether a number was smaller or larger than a predetermined reference number by pressing one of two response buttons, it was found that ‘large’ responses were faster with the right hand than with the left hand. The reverse was true for small numbers.

In a study investigating the mental processing of parity judgement, Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993) provided further evidence concerning the response-side effect (and called it SNARC effect for Spatial Numerical Association of Response Codes). In evaluating a number's parity status, shorter latencies were obtained for small numbers responded to with the left hand than with the right hand. Conversely, large numbers were responded to preferentially with the right hand. Because parity is a numerical property which alternates between consecutive numbers, this study extends the response-side effect by showing that the effect is not specific to the stimulus-response mapping of the comparison task. It also follows that the effect is not related to the use of a reference number for comparison.

Importantly, the results also show that the numerical value is automatically activated, because in principle the parity judgement task needs no access to numerical magnitude information for correct task performance. Further support for the possibility of automatic activation of spatially coded numerical magnitudes comes from a series of experiments which showed that the SNARC effect was elicited, even when digits served merely as a background distracter with a non-numerical target superimposed in an orientation discrimination task (Fias, Lauwereyns, & Lammertyn, 2001).

In the present paper we investigate whether a similar association exists between non-numerical ordered sequences and spatial properties of the response. Dehaene et al. (1993; Experiment 4) already used letters in situations that were structurally analogous to the parity judgement task. In a first condition subjects had to classify the letters A–F into the categories ACE or BDF. No SNARC effect was obtained. One could argue, however, that the categories along which the letters had to be classified were arbitrary. This may have induced an atypical way of performing the task. This assumption is supported by the fact that the pattern of response times (RTs) was compatible with a serial search strategy. In a second condition, letters were used in a consonant–vowel classification task spanning almost the entire alphabet, but a significant SNARC effect was not obtained. However, there was a tendency toward a SNARC effect. Given the small number of subjects (n=10), one could argue that the experiment did not have enough statistical power. Most importantly, in both conditions the ordering of the stimuli was irrelevant to the task. One could imagine that the mental representation of order is spatially organized, but that, unlike numbers, this spatially organized representation is not accessed automatically. Taken together, there are a number of reasons why the conditions to test a possible spatial association of order and space were suboptimal.

In the following experiments we investigate the possible spatial organization of two ordered sequences, namely months of the year (Experiment 1) and letters (Experiment 2). For each stimulus type, subjects performed both an order-relevant task (comparison with a fixed standard) and an order-irrelevant task (letter detection in Experiment 1 and consonant–vowel classification in Experiment 2).

The order-relevant task allows judging of the spatial coding of internally ordered information, and with the order-irrelevant task the automaticity of activation of the spatial codes can be evaluated.

Section snippets

Method

Twenty-five Dutch speaking participants (four left-handed, mean age: 21.4 years) completed both order-relevant and -irrelevant tasks. The range of stimuli consisted of eight months, ranging from January to April and from September to December, presented in Dutch (“januari”, “februari”, “maart”, “april”, “september”, “oktober”, “november” and “december”). During the order-relevant task, subjects were asked to judge the position of the months as coming before or after July. During the

Experiment 2: letters

Although the results of Experiment 1 are quite clear, one could argue that the conclusion of spatial coding of ordinal sequences is not justified because it is common practice to numerically code the months of the year. Thus, in principle it is possible that the spatial coding derives from numerical recoding. While numerical recoding is indeed well-learned in the case of months, it is not a feasible option for other sequences. Assigning numbers to the letters of the alphabet, for instance, has

General discussion

In line with the findings of Dehaene et al., 1993, Dehaene et al., 1990 in the domain of numbers, we obtained clear evidence that the mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially coded. In Experiment 1 we found that months from the beginning of the year are responded to faster with the left hand than with the right hand, whereas the reverse pattern was obtained for months towards the end of the year. This was true not only in a task in which ordinal position had to be compared to a

Acknowledgements

This research is supported by IUAP P5/04 and by grant D.0353.01 of the Flemish Fund for Scientific Research. Bert Reynvoet is a post-doc researcher supported by the Flemish Fund for Scientific Research. The authors wish to thank Sam Gilbert for checking the English.

References (11)

  • S. Dehaene et al.

    The mental representation of parity and number magnitude

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1993)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic

    Cortex

    (1997)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1990)
  • W. Fias et al.

    The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: evidence from the SNARC effect

    Mathematical Cognition

    (1996)
  • W. Fias et al.

    Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2001)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (370)

  • A sensorimotor perspective on numerical cognition

    2023, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text