Elsevier

Sleep Health

Volume 3, Issue 1, February 2017, Pages 22-27
Sleep Health

Social support, social strain, sleep quality, and actigraphic sleep characteristics: evidence from a national survey of US adults

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2016.10.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

To determine the associations between average family and friend social support and strain over 10 years and sleep quality, sleep efficiency, total sleep time, and night-to-night total sleep time variability.

Participants

Non-institutionalized English-speaking US adults aged 34ā€“81 who participated in the MacArthur Study on Aging: Midlife in the United States.

Measurements

Sleep quality was assessed by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and by a 7-day daily diary. Sleep efficiency, total sleep time, and night-to-night total sleep time variability were assessed by actigraphy (MiniMitter 64).

Results

Social support, but not social strain, was significantly associated with both self-reported measures of quality (social support Ī²Ā =Ā āˆ’1.239, PĀ =Ā .019 for global Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scores; social support Ī²Ā =Ā āˆ’0.248, PĀ =Ā .016 for diary assessed quality). Lower scores on both quality measures indicate better sleep. In contrast, social strain, but not social support, was significantly associated with sleep efficiency (social strain Ī²Ā =Ā āˆ’3.780, PĀ =Ā .007). Social strain, but not social support, was significantly associated with night-to-night sleep variability (social strain Ī²Ā =Ā 0.421, PĀ =Ā .034); however, the overall model was not significant. Neither social support nor social strain was significantly associated with total sleep time.

Conclusion

Social support was significant for self-reported sleep, whereas only social strain was significantly associated with objective sleep parameters. Future research on social relationships and sleep should analyze both positive and negative aspects of relationships in tandem because effects appear to differ based on outcome.

Introduction

The consequences of poor sleep are increasingly understood to affect health, from mortality risk to cardiovascular disease, obesity, diabetes, and many others (see Czeisler1 [2015] for a summary).1, 2, 3, 4, 5 The significance of sleep to health begs the question: What contributes to poor sleep? The determinants of sleep can be found at different levels of analysis, from the genetic to the social. This article operates at the social psychological level to understand how social support and social strain from family and friends may impact both subjective and objective sleep characteristics. The sleep literature at the social psychological level tends to consider 3 aspects of social relationships: social support, loneliness, and social strain, which are operationally defined as perceptions of the supportive, lacking (in connection), or strained aspects of the individual's social network.

The first aspect is social support. Seminal work by Cassel and Cobb in the 1970s established social support as a significant protective factor for a variety of health outcomes.6, 7, 8 These protective effects appear to hold for many aspects of sleep; conversely, a lack of social support is predictive of poor sleep. Low social support is associated with increased odds of shorter self-reported sleep duration, whether duration is operationalized as ā‰¤6 hours, ā‰¤7 hours, or perceived days of insufficient sleep per week.9, 10, 11 When sleep was assessed by actigraphy, however, different results were obtained: emotional support was not predictive of total sleep time (TST) (or sleep quality) but was predictive of lesser wake after sleep onset.12 Despite some differences between subjective and objective sleep outcomes for TST, it appears that supportive social relationships generally have a positive effect on sleep.

Supportive social relationships are thus highly desirable. When people want social connectedness and yet have their wishes frustrated, the result is conceptualized as loneliness. Loneliness, the second aspect of social relationships, is defined as a perception of a lack of social connection. Loneliness contributes to poor sleep efficiency (SE), poor daytime function, and sleep fragmentation but not sleep duration.13, 14, 15 The mechanism by which loneliness affects sleep may include ā€œfeelings of vulnerability and unconscious vigilance for social threat, implicit cognitions that are antithetical to relaxation and sound sleep.ā€16 (p4)

However, as desirable as relationships may be, relationships can themselves be a source of strain. Thus, a third approach builds on the social support literature to include the negative aspects of social relationships. It is important to note that the presence of strain does not necessarily imply the absence of support, for there is evidence to suggest that social support and strain are independent.17, 18 Because a lack of social support is not the same as the presence of strain, analysis of support alone would yield a partial understanding of the effect of social relationships on sleep. To date, however, few articles on sleep have included social strain.19, 20 This appears to be an oversight because the literature indicates that the effects of the negative aspects of social relationships on well-being generally tend to be either as powerful or even more so than the positive aspects of social relationships.21 If this proposition holds for sleep, then negative aspects of social relationships may have a greater effect on sleep parameters.

This third line of research typically analyzes support and strain together. It consistently finds that negative aspects of relationships influence sleep. High levels of family strain and low levels of family support produce the highest odds of reporting weekly/daily sleep problems.19 Interpersonal distress is correlated with sleep and arousal.22 Aversive social ties correlate with poorer self-reported sleep quality, and supportive ties correlate with better sleep quality, with depression as a significant mediator.20

Strides have thus been made toward a fuller understanding of how social relationships affect self-reported sleep. However, it is well-known that self-reported and objectively measured sleep outcomes often yield different results, which suggests that they may be distinct phenomena deserving separate analysis.23, 24, 25 In addition, some aspects of self-reported sleep such as global sleep quality and sleep problems do not have straightforward objective analogues. Thus, the literature has left open to investigation whether social support and strain are associated with objectively measured sleep parameters such as TST, SE, and night-to-night variability in TST, a parameter of increasing interest due to its association with depressive symptoms and subjective well-being.26, 27 Furthermore, it is unknown whether social support or strain will have the larger effect on objective sleep parameters and if results differ with self-reported sleep. A study employing both objective and subjective sleep outcomes may provide a clearer picture of the effects of social relationships on sleep.

Thus, the questions that motivate this study are: What are the contributions of social support and social strain to sleep quality, efficiency, TST, and night-to-night TST variability? Which has the greater effect on sleep: social support or social strain? It is hypothesized that support should be predictive of higher sleep quality, SE, TST, and lower night-to-night TST variability. Social strain, on the other hand, should be predictive of lower sleep quality, SE, TST, and higher night-to-night TST variability. However, if a comparative claim can be made, strain may have the larger effect, consistent with the literature on well-being.21 This article thus considers the associations between positive and negative aspects of social relationships and sleep using multiple objective sleep parameters. Furthermore, this article analyzes sleep in a subset of a national probability sample, which is demographically diverse in age, sex, and marital status.

Section snippets

Participants and methods

Data are drawn from the MacArthur study on Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS), a national probability sample of noninstitutionalized English-speaking adults in the contiguous United States obtained by random-digit-dialing, aged 34-84 at wave II. Of the several waves, the first and second waves of MIDUS (1994-1995 and 2004-2006) and the Biomarker supplement (2004-009) are used. Of the 7108 respondents at wave I, 4963 also responded at wave II. A subsample of this population, 1255

Results

For the PSQI, social support is significant (Ī²Ā =Ā āˆ’1.239, PĀ =Ā .019; Table 2). The 95% confidence interval indicates that with each unit increase in social support, a respondent's PSQI score is plausibly expected to decrease (better quality) anywhere from 0.206 to 2.271 units. Social strain is not significant (Ī²Ā =Ā 0.079, PĀ =Ā .876). This model accounts for approximately 20.33% of the variance (FĀ =Ā 4.01, PĀ <Ā .001, nĀ =Ā 236).

For average daily diary reported sleep quality, social support is significant (Ī²Ā =Ā āˆ’0.248, PĀ =

Discussion

We are now in a position to evaluate these results. The results of this analysis suggest that social support, but not social strain, is consequential for subjectively assessed sleep quality. This partially supports the findings of Ailshire and Burgard19 (2013) and Kent et al 20 (2015) that both support and strain contribute to self-reported sleep problems or quality.2

Disclosure

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to the anonymous reviewers of Sleep Health and Rick Rubinson, Ellen Idler, and Tim Dowd for helpful comments on an earlier draft; Samantha Noreen for critical feedback on methods; Dan Buysse and Nancy Collop on information for the PSQI; and Ann Rogers for information on actigraphy. Any deficiencies in this article are my own.

The MIDUS 1 study (Midlife in the United States) was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife

References (44)

  • M.A. Dew et al.

    Healthy older adults' sleep predicts all-cause mortality at 4 to 19 years of follow-up

    Psychosom Med

    (2003)
  • C.L. Jackson et al.

    Association between sleep duration and diabetes in black and white adults

    Diabetes Care

    (2013)
  • J. Cassel

    The contribution of the social environment to host resistance

    Am J Epidemiol

    (1976)
  • J.S. House et al.

    Social relationships and health

    Science

    (1988)
  • S. Cobb

    Social support as a moderator of life stress

    Psychosom Med

    (1976)
  • C. Glenn et al.

    Social support and sleep symptoms in US adults

    Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine

    (2015)
  • M.A. Grandner et al.

    Social and behavioral determinants of perceived insufficient sleep

    Front Neurol

    (2015)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Do lonely days invade the nights? Potential social modulation of sleep efficiency

    Psychol Sci

    (2002)
  • L.C. Hawkley et al.

    Loneliness impairs daytime functioning but not sleep duration

    Health Psychol

    (2010)
  • L.M. Kurina et al.

    Loneliness is associated with sleep fragmentation in a communal society

    Sleep

    (2011)
  • L.C. Hawkley et al.

    Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms

    Ann Behav Med

    (2010)
  • J.F. Finch et al.

    Positive and negative social ties among older adults: measurement models and the prediction of psychological distress and well-being

    Am J Community Psychol

    (1989)
  • Cited by (47)

    • Psychosocial dimensions of sleep health

      2022, Foundations of Sleep Health
    • Sleep in times of crises: A scoping review in the early days of the COVID-19 crisis

      2021, Sleep Medicine Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      In studies that divided social support into subcategories, emotional support was the component most strongly associated with better self-reported sleep outcomes [80]. However, it is important to mention that some studies found no effect of social support on objective measures of sleep [81,82]. In general, social support, in counteracting loneliness, has beneficial effects on both sleep, general health [52] and depression [49].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text