Skip to content
Publicly Available Published by De Gruyter October 1, 2016

A meta-analysis of fear-avoidance and pain intensity: The paradox of chronic pain

  • Emily B. Kroska EMAIL logo

Abstract

Background

The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain has established avoidance as a predictor of negative outcomes in chronic pain patients. Avoidance, or deliberate attempts to suppress or prevent unwanted experiences (e.g., pain), has been studied extensively, with multiple reviews implicating this behavior as a predictor of disability, physical disuse, and depression. Despite hundreds of studies examining the associations between different components of this model (i.e., catastrophizing, fear, avoidance, depression), the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity has remained unclear. The present study seeks to clarify this association across samples.

Method

The present analyses synthesize the literature (articles from PsycInfo, PubMed, and ProQuest) to determine if fear-avoidance and pain intensity are consistently correlated across studies, samples, and measures. Eligible studies measured pain intensity and fear-avoidance cross-sectionally in chronic pain patients. The search resulted in 118 studies eligible for inclusion. A random-effects model was used to estimate the weighted mean effect size. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software was used for all analyses. Moderation analyses elucidate the variables that affect the strength of this association. Meta-regression and meta-ANOVA analyses were conducted to examine moderating variables. Moderator variables include demographic characteristics, pain characteristics, study characteristics, and national cultural characteristics (using Hofstede’s cultural dimensions). Publication bias was examined using the funnel plot and the p-curve.

Results

Results indicate a small-to-moderate positive association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. The results were stable across characteristics of the sample, including mean age, gender distribution, marital status, and duration of pain. Moderation analyses indicate that the measures utilized and cultural differences affect the strength of this association. Weaker effect sizes were observed for studies that utilized measures of experiential avoidance when compared to studies that utilized pain-specific fear-avoidance measures. Studies that utilized multiple measures of fear-avoidance had stronger effect sizes than studies that utilized a single measure of fear-avoidance. Three of Hofstede’s cultural dimensions moderated the association, including Power Distance Index, Individualism versus Collectivism, and Indulgence versus Restraint.

Conclusions

The present meta-analysis synthesizes the results from studies examining the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity among individuals with chronic pain. The positive association indicates that those with increased fear-avoidance have higher pain intensity, and those with higher pain intensity have increased fear-avoidance. Findings indicate that cultural differences and measurement instruments are important to consider in understanding the variables that affect this association. The significant cultural variations may indicate that it is important to consider the function of avoidance behavior in different cultures in an effort to better understand each patient’s cultural beliefs, as well as how these beliefs are related to pain and associated coping strategies.

Implications

The results from the current meta-analysis can be used to inform interventions for patients with chronic pain. In particular, those with more intense pain or increased fear-avoidance should be targeted for prevention and intervention work. Within the intervention itself, avoidance should be undermined and established as an ineffective strategy to manage pain in an effort to prevent disability, depression, and physical deconditioning.

1 Introduction

Chronic pain affects about 20% of the global population [1]. Those afflicted with chronic pain are at an increased risk for poor work productivity, disability, unemployment, and poor social and emotional functioning [2, 3]. Increasing concern about the prevalence of chronic pain and associated emotional suffering has driven research on the etiology and treatment of chronic pain conditions.

Across the chronic pain literature, distinct coping strategies have been identified. A well-established response to pain is fear, and a common response to fear is avoidance [2]. Avoidance is any attempt to reduce or prevent unwanted experiences. Avoidance may effectively reduce fear and pain intensity in the short-term. In the long-term, however, avoiding physiological sensations associated with chronic pain and movement may result in physical deconditioning, which may then lead to more pain when mobile.

The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain has been examined in hundreds of studies over the past 30 years [4] providing a framework for understanding the psychological factors that impact outcomes in chronic pain patients. The fear-avoidance model posits that in response to pain, catastrophizing brings about fear, followed by avoidance behaviors (see Fig. 1). Those who adopt this model refer to this construct as fear-avoidance.

Fig. 1 
						Fear-avoidance model of chronic pain.
Fig. 1

Fear-avoidance model of chronic pain.

Many studies have indicated that fear-avoidance increases disability and self-reported suffering [5, 6, 7, 8]. A recent meta-analysis indicated that across studies, pain-related fear and disability were modestly associated (r = .50; [9]), and pain intensity did not significantly moderate the association. Despite several studies indicating a positive association between pain intensity and disability [10, 11, 12], the fear-avoidance model posits that within individuals who experience high pain intensity, the level of disability is heavily dependent upon avoidance [13]. Thus, pain intensity is related to disability, but behavior in response to pain is a better predictor of disability [14]. Thus, the fear-avoidance model posits fear-avoidance as a key ingredient in predicting disability, yet the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity is less clear. Although research of the fear-avoidance model was designed to measure disability, pain intensity is an additional important variable. The current review seeks to understand how pain intensity impacts fear-avoidance to facilitate a more comprehensive picture of the psychological factors involved in chronic pain.

Several studies have indicated a positive association between reported pain intensity and fear-avoidance [15, 16]. If those with higher pain intensity are more prone to fear-avoidance, this provides an opportunity for intervention. The current literature suggests that fear-avoidance is associated with negative outcomes in chronic pain populations and predicts disability. Thus, there is a need for interventions that seek to increase willingness to experience pain and engage in valued activities in the presence of intense pain.

1.1 Objectives of the current meta-analysis

The present study synthesizes the findings from studies across different samples and measures and determines the strength of the association between pain intensity and fear-avoidance. Furthermore, moderation analyses elucidate other variables that influence the strength of this association.

2 Method

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA; [17]) checklist was used in preparing and conducting the current meta-analysis.

2.1 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies were required to include a human sample of individuals with chronic pain (≥three months duration). Studies were excluded if they included individuals with acute pain–that is, those who reported pain duration of less than three months. In the event that a study explicitly stated that the majority of participants reported chronic pain, the author was contacted to provide data with only the chronic pain patients.

Additionally, included studies must have been published in English. Furthermore, only empirical studies were examined. Studies were also required to utilize original data in order to avoid including the same effect size more than once. In the event that more than one study was published using the same data, the data were included once, utilizing the first study to report the necessary information for moderation analyses. Qualitative data were not included in the present analyses. No restrictions were placed on date of publication. Moreover, it was necessary that studies utilized a self-report measure of both fear-avoidance and pain intensity. Studies were included if they reported correlation(s) between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. In the event that the study was longitudinal or an intervention trial, only baseline measures were utilized to avoid confounding the association of interest with change over time as a result of the intervention. If a study included both a measure of fear-avoidance and pain intensity and met all other inclusion criteria, but did not report sufficient data to calculate a correlation, the authors were contacted to provide the data or relevant statistics.

2.2 Selection of predictor variables

Across the literature, multiple measures of fear-avoidance have been utilized. Most of these measures confound fear and avoidance, using items that address both constructs in the same scale (e.g., Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire, Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale). As a result, the current meta-analysis included all measures of fear-avoidance–those that measure avoidance exclusively as well as those that measure fear and avoidance. Although these were included in an effort to provide the most comprehensive representation of the current literature, the over-arching issue still remains. Fear and avoidance are distinct theoretical concepts, addressing two different parts of the fear-avoidance model. Fear is the physiological response to pain, as the body perceives pain to be threatening. Avoidance is the behavioral response to this pain–any attempt to deliberately reduce or prevent the unwanted experience. The combination of these two constructs, in theory or in measurement instruments, confounds associations between fear or avoidance and other variables of interest (e.g., pain intensity, disability).

Experiential avoidance is frequently measured using self-report instruments that evaluate fear or avoidance of unwanted or painful sensations, emotions, thoughts, or other experiences. Although some measures of avoidance focus explicitly on pain-specific fear and avoidance, others focus more broadly on avoidance of any stimuli [18, 19]. Furthermore, a variety of problematic outcomes are known to be associated with experiential avoidance, including increased psychiatric symptoms [20, 21]. This adds to the variability seen in the association between pain intensity and fear-avoidance. The current meta-analysis includes measures that focus specifically on pain-related fear-avoidance, as well as those that measure fear-avoidance of stimuli more generally.

Furthermore, some measures of avoidance are frequently used as measures of acceptance after being reverse-scored (e.g., Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; [22, 23]). In addition, measures of acceptance often represent lower levels of avoidance as higher levels of acceptance. For example, the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire has two subscales, one of which (pain willingness) measures avoidance and is reverse-scored so that higher scores on this subscale represent greater acceptance [24]. For this reason, studies that included measures of acceptance were included, and the effect size was reverse-coded.

Studies that included one or more measures of fear-avoidance were eligible for inclusion. In the event that more than one measure was included, all reported effect sizes were averaged in an effort to represent all measures. Composite effect sizes across all measures of fear-avoidance were utilized to form the most representative weighted effect size. Subsequently, composite effect sizes across only pain-specific fear-avoidance measures were calculated, and these were used to estimate the effect size between pain-specific fear-avoidance and pain intensity (referenced in results as “Pain-Specific Fear-Avoidance”). Effect sizes were calculated for studies that utilized measures of fear-avoidance of any stimuli (referenced in results as “General Fear-Avoidance”), and this weighted effect size represents the association between experiential avoidance and pain intensity. Furthermore, if subscale and total scores were both used in the correlation analyses, total scores were used. If only subscale scores were utilized, then all subscale-based effect sizes were averaged.

Pain intensity measures were self-report scales that asked participants to rate the intensity or severity of pain. Most measures ask participants to rate the intensity of pain currently, though others will average the ratings of worst pain, least pain, average pain, and current pain. A substantial number of included studies utilized a one-item visual analog or numerical rating scale to measure pain intensity. Table 1 presents a summary of the fear-avoidance measures included, and Table 2 presents the pain intensity measures that were included in the analyses.

Table 1

Included fear-avoidance measures and domains assessed.

Measure Domains assessed
Pain-specific fear-ivoidance measures
 TSK [142] Focus on somatic symptoms and avoidance of activities that could result in pain
 CPAQ [24] Willingness to experience pain and engage in activities
 FABQ [19] Fear-avoidance beliefs in physical and work activities
 PASS[143] Cognitive anxiety, physiological anxiety, escape/avoidance behaviors, fearof pain
 PBQ[144] Avoidance, compliance, and help-seeking behaviors used in response to pain
 FOPQ [145] Pain-related fears and avoidance behaviors
 FAPS [146] Degree of avoidance associated with situations or activities
 POAM-P [147] Frequency of avoidance and persistence behavior
 FSR[148] Avoidance, resignation, and distraction in response to pain
 DICI [149] Illness-related coping activities
 ACPT [44] Acceptance of pain
 PBC[150] Avoidance of activities due to pain
 TOPS [151] Fear-avoidance behaviors associated with chronic pain
 PARQ [152] Avoidance, pacing, and confronting behavior in chronic pain
 BCPI [153] Psychological flexibility coping strategies and other traditional coping strategies
 PCI[154] Retreating and resting attempts to limit pain
 PSITQ [155] Frequency ofavoidance ofactivities due to pain
 PSI [156] Perceptions of personal problem-solving behaviors and attitudes
General fear-avoidance measures
 AAQ [18] Experiential avoidance ofthoughts, emotions, situations, or experiences
 CISS [157] Frequency ofengaging in various coping activities during stressful situations
 WOC [158] Ways of coping; escape-avoidance subscale
  1. Abbreviations: TSK, Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia; CPAQ Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire; FABQ Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire; PASS, Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale; PBQ Pain Behavior Questionnaire; FOPQ Fear of Pain Questionnaire; FAPS, Fear Avoidance of Pain Scale; POAM-P, Patterns of Activity Mapping–Pain; FSR, Questionnaire on Pain Regulation; DICI, Dealing with Illness Coping Inventory; ACPT, Acceptance and Control in Psychological Treatment; PBC, Pain Behavior Questionnaire; TOPS, Treatment Outcomes of Pain Survey; PARQ, Pain and Activity Relations Questionnaire; BCPI, Brief Pain Coping Inventory; PCI, Pain Coping Inventory; PSITQ; Pain Situations Questionnaire; PSI, Problem-Solving Inventory; AAQ, Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; CISS, Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations; WOC, Ways of Coping.

Table 2

Included pain intensity measures and domains assessed.

Measure Domains assessed
Pain intensity measures
 VAS 100 mm line from no paintoworst pain imaginable to measure current pain
 NRS [159] Current pain rated on numeric scale from no pain to worst pain imaginable
 MPQ [160] Checklist of adjectives to describe pain; subscale to rate pain severity
 MPI [161] Severity of pain rated on seven-point scale, three items
 BPI [162] Pain severity associated with variety of activities
 PII [163] Average of the highest, lowest, average, and current level of pain
 BAP [164] Psychological and physical functioning and pain intensity
 HI [165] Average daily headache score
 TOPS [151] Pain symptoms, among other subscales
 SF-36 [166] Bodily pain severity, among other subscales
 HQ [5, 6] Severity oftypical headache
 VRS [167] Verbal rating scale of pain intensity
 PINRS [168] Pain severity over the last two weeks
 GCPS [169] Current pain, worst pain in last 6 months, and average pain in last 6 months
 PIR [170] Numeric rating scale for pain intensity
 AIMS-2 [171] Physical, social, and emotional functioning of RA patients
  1. Abbreviations: VAS, Visual Analogue Scale; NRS, Numerical Rating Scale; MPQ McGill Pain Questionnaire; MPI, Multidimensional Pain Inventory; BPI, Brief Pain Inventory; PII, Pain Intensity Index; BAP, Brief Assessment of Pain; HI, Headache Index; TOPS, Treatment Outcomes of Pain Survey; SF-36, Short Form Health Survey-36; HQ, Headache Questionnaire; VRS, Verbal Rating Scale; PINRS, Pain Intensity Numeric Rating Scale; GCPS, Graded Chronic Pain Scale; PIR, Pain Intensity Rating; AIMS-2, Arthritis Impact Scale-2nd edition.

2.3 Moderator variables

Moderator variables included in the present analysis span demographic characteristics, pain characteristics, and study characteristics. Data were collected from available studies or provided by authors in the case that data were not reported in the article. To test for moderation within demographic characteristics, mean age, percentage of males in the sample, and percentage of married or cohabiting individuals in the sample were examined as moderator variables. The central tendency (mean or median) of pain duration in the sample was examined as a moderator as well. Lastly, study characteristics were examined. Studies that included measures of pain-specific fear-avoidance, general fear-avoidance, or both of these measures were compared. Next, the type of sample recruited (e.g., chronic back pain, fibromyalgia) was examined as a moderator if there were three or more like samples.

The country from which participants were recruited was also recorded. The Hofstede dimensions of national culture were then utilized to examine culture as a moderator. These dimensions have been used extensively as a cultural comparison tool [25], based on an initial examination of 116,000 questionnaires given in countries around the world and many subsequent replications that found these same dimensions [26]. The dimensions include the Power Distance Index (PDI), Individualism versus Collectivism (IDV), Masculinity versus Femininity (MAS), Uncertainty Avoidance Index (UAI), Long Term Orientation versus Short Term Normative Orientation (LTO), and Indulgence versus Restraint (IND).The PDI reflects the way in which a society feels and acts regarding inequality, with higher scores representing acceptance of inequalities among people. The IDV index expresses the extent to which individualism (“I” mentality) and collectivism (“we” mentality) are balanced within a culture. Higher scores on the IDV index indicate higher levels of individualism. The MAS dimension defines masculinity in society as a preference for achievement and competition, whereas femininity is defined as a preference for cooperation and consensus. Higher scores on the MAS dimension indicate more masculine preferences. The UAI reflects how comfortable a society is with uncertainty, with higher scores representing intolerance of uncertainty. The LTO index quantifies the balance between managing past challenges and handling present and future challenges. Higher scores on this dimension indicate preferences for societal change and working toward the future. Finally, the IND index represents the extent to which short-term gratification and long-term gratification are valued in a society. Higher scores on this dimension indicate greater value on indulgence and immediate gratification. Though Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture have been used considerably [25], the validity of this measure has been questioned [27].

2.4 Search strategies

Searches were conducted using PsycInfo, PubMed, and ProQuest databases. Search terms included “avoidance” and “chronic pain” for studies published in English at any time prior to the final search date (January 26, 2016). In the case of ProQuest dissertations and theses, the same search terms were used, but the search was limited to abstracts due to the database’s incorporation of all9 disciplines.

For all searches, an initial screen was completed to exclude studies that did not meet the inclusion criteria. Subsequently, the abstracts of all remaining articles were inspected, and if the article appeared to meet the inclusion criteria, the full article was obtained. Then, reference lists from each included article were examined for articles missed during the initial search. After excluding repetitions across search engines, this process identified 799 studies for consideration. Eight of these studies were not available via the Internet, hard copy, or interlibrary loan (k = 8). One study was not included because it had been retracted since publication (k =1). Four studies were excluded because the studies were not quantitative in nature (k = 4). Of all available studies for inclusion, studies were excluded because they were not empirical studies (k = 155), did not use human subjects (k = 29), or were not available in English (k = 45). Furthermore, studies were excluded because they did not recruit from a chronic pain population (k = 168) or did not include self-report, cross-sectional measures of fear-avoidance or pain intensity (k = 138). Others were excluded if they did not report sufficient information to calculate effect sizes, or reported effects sizes were confounded (k = 126). Studies were excluded if they reported non-independent data (k = 7). With these exclusions, a total of 118 studies were included in the present meta-analysis.

Considerable effort was put forth to obtain unpublished data by contacting listservs (e.g., Association for Cognitive and Behavioral Therapies, Society for Behavioral Medicine’s Pain Special Interest Group) and searching conference proceedings using all-academic.com. No unpublished data were obtained.

2.5 Coding procedures

The first author coded the studies with the help of a postbaccalaureate research assistant. To assess intercoder agreement, the first author and research assistant double coded a random sample of 30% of the included studies. The coder was trained to reach acceptable reliability. According to Fleiss’ [28] standards, a kappa of .75 or greater indicates excellent agreement. The kappa for the present meta-analysis was .87, which indicates excellent agreement. All discrepancies were resolved through discussion until 100% agreement was reached.

The following variables were coded from each study provided the information was available: (a) design of the study (crosssectional, longitudinal), (b) type of sample (e.g., chronic low back pain, fibromyalgia), (c) N of sample, (d) age of sample (M, SD), (e) percent of males in sample, (f) percent of sample married/cohabiting, (g) country where study was conducted, (h) central tendency of pain duration in months (mean, median), (i) measure(s) of fear-avoidance, pain and depression, (j) reliability coefficients for fear-avoidance, pain, and depression measures, and (k) correlation(s) between fear-avoidance and pain intensity.

2.6 Meta-analytic procedure

All effect size computations were made using Fisher’s Z transformations, with study weights of n–3 [29]. The computations were completed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software [30].

Preliminary analyses of effect sizes were conducted. Effect sizes were examined for outliers. Analyses to detect publication bias were conducted to determine whether studies with small effect sizes or nonsignificant results had a lower probability of getting published. Trim-and-fill analyses (executed in the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software package; [31]) and funnel plots were examined for evidence of publication bias (see Fig. 2). A funnel plot visually displays publication bias estimates using sample size and effect size [32]. A symmetrical funnel plot indicates a lack of publication bias. Because several examinations of the funnel plot have indicated that this method falsely concludes publication bias when it is in fact not present [33, 34], the p-curve was used as an additional measure of publication bias. The p-curve is a method used to model the distribution of significant p values [35]. The trim-and-fill analyses assume that studies with small effect sizes are less likely to be published, and the p-curve assumes that studies with nonsignificant results are less likely to be published, regardless of the effect size [36]. Both approaches were utilized in order to address both perspectives. All effect size and sample size data were entered into the online application at http://p-curve.org/ to generate a p-curve and the relevant statistics.

Fig. 2 
							Funnel plot indicating the relationship between effect size and standard error (publication bias) in studies examining the association between fear-avoidance and pain. The hollow diamond indicates the mean effect size, and hollow circles indicate a single effect size. There are no black diamonds, indicating no imputed effect sizes.
Fig. 2

Funnel plot indicating the relationship between effect size and standard error (publication bias) in studies examining the association between fear-avoidance and pain. The hollow diamond indicates the mean effect size, and hollow circles indicate a single effect size. There are no black diamonds, indicating no imputed effect sizes.

A random-effects model was utilized to estimate a weighted mean effect size [37]. A fixed-effects model assumes that the effect sizes across diverse samples will be the same. Given the observed diversity in the strength of the association between pain intensity and fear-avoidance across the literature, a random-effects model was most appropriate. A weighted mean effect size was computed using CMA. Effect size distributions were tested for homogeneity. Meta-regression and meta-ANOVA were used to examine moderators of the weighted mean effect size.

3 Results

3.1 Description of studies

The 118 included studies in this meta-analysis were published between 1989 and 2015. The total number of participants included in the present analyses is 25,969. Sample sizes varied between 8 and 6147 (M = 220.08, SD = 567.77). On average, the samples were made up of 34.92% males (range: 0–100%), with two studies that did not report gender distribution. The mean age of participants was 43.92 years (SD = 12.05), with three samples that did not report a mean age. All samples utilized a chronic pain population with various types of chronic pain: 69 nonspecific chronic pain, 30 chronic back pain, 4 fibromyalgia, 3 chronic headache, 4 chronic spinal pain, 2 rheumatoid arthritis, 2 temporomandibular facial pain, 1 endometriosis, 1 irritable bowel disease, 1 chronic migraine, 3 multiple sclerosis, 1 osteoarthritis, 1 work-related chronic pain, and 1 whiplash associated disorder. The average percentage of the samples that were married or cohabiting was 63.48% (range: 6–84%), with 67 studies reporting this information. The average pain duration of the samples was 84.22 months (range: 12–201.84), with 94 studies reporting the central tendency (mean or median) of pain duration. Appendix A shows the sample characteristics and effect sizes for all included studies.

A variety of measures of fear-avoidance were used in the included studies, with many of the studies (38.13%) using multiple measures. Table 1 provides a summary of the included measures, as well as the domains assessed by these measures. Pain intensity was also assessed using a variety of measures. The pain intensity measures from included studies are summarized in Table 2.

3.2 Preliminary analyses

The data indicated that there were no statistical outliers in the effect sizes. As a result, all studies were included in analyses.

3.3 Effect size analyses

Weighted mean effect sizes were computed for the crosssectional association between fear-avoidance and pain (see Table 3). The total effect size represents the weighted mean effect size calculated using the average across all reported correlations between fear-avoidance and pain intensity, including both pain-specific and general avoidance measures. The results of the effect size analyses supported a positive association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity (with a weighted mean effect size of r = .26). Thus, as pain intensity increases, fear-avoidance also increases. As shown in Table 3, the Q statistic (a measure of homogeneity) was statistically significant, indicating a large ratio of between-study to within-study variance [38], Q(118) = 710.25, p < .001. I2 analyses were used to quantify inconsistency and heterogeneity in the results. The I2 statistic represents the percentage of variation due to heterogeneity rather than chance [39]. The I2 value computed herein (I2 =83.53) indicates considerable heterogeneity and signifies that a substantial amount of variation across included studies is due to heterogeneity. Thus, moderation analyses are justified.

Table 3

Effect size r estimates for association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity.

Moderator r 95% CI z k Q I 2
Total .26 .23–.30 15.87[***] 118 710.25[***] 83.53
Pain-specific fear-avoidance .28 .25–.31 16.16[***] 111 708.62[***] 84.48
General fear-avoidance .18 .09–.27 3.89[***] 15 65.53[***] 78.64
  1. Note. Estimates utilized a random effects model. Values in the Q column indicate the heterogeneity in the weighted mean effect size. CI = confidence interval.

4 Pain-specific fear-avoidance vs. general fear-avoidance effect sizes

Weighted mean effect sizes were computed for the associations between pain-specific fear-avoidance and pain intensity, as well as general fear-avoidance and pain intensity. The weighted mean effect size for pain-specific fear-avoidance and pain intensity indicates a positive association (r = .28), and the weighted mean effect size for general fear-avoidance and pain intensity also supports a positive association (r = .18).

4.1 Moderation analyses

The previous analyses indicated heterogeneity in the distribution of effect sizes. As a result, moderation analyses were conducted. First, meta-regression analyses were undertaken to determine if pain duration moderated the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity (see Table 4). The metaregression analysis indicated that pain duration did not moderate this association, Q(93) = .47, p > .05. Thus, duration of pain does not affect the strength of the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity.

Table 4

Tests of meta-regression for the relation between fear-avoidance and pain intensity.

Moderator Point estimate 95% CI z k Q
Total .26[***] .23 to −.29 15.00[***] 112 691.42[***]
 Pain Duration .0002 −.0004 to .0003 0.68 93 .47
 Percent Married .0024 −.0006 to .0053 1.59 66 2.51
 Age .0024 −.0001 to .0049 1.92 114 3.67
 Percent Male .0005 −.0004 to .0015 1.07 115 1.14
 PDI .0032 .0008 to .0055 2.66[**] 116 7.06[**]
 IDV −.0026 −.0039to −.0013 -3.89[***] 116 15.15[***]
 MAS −.0010 −.0022to.0003 -1.46 116 2.12
 UAI .0011 −.0003 to .0024 1.57 116 2.46
 LTO .0006 −.0007 to .0019 .90 116 .82
 IND −.0025 −.0043 to −.0006 -2.64[*] 115 6.95[*]
  1. Note. Values in the Q column indicate heterogeneity in a variable.

    Abbreviations: CI = confidence interval. PDI, power distance index; IDV, individualism versus collectivism; MAS, masculinity versus feminity; UAI, uncertainty avoidance index; LTO, long term orientation versus short term normative orientation; IND, indulgence versus restraint.

Other sample characteristics were examined as moderators of the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. The percentage of the sample that were married or cohabiting did not significantly moderate the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity, Q(66) = 2.51, p > .05. Furthermore, the mean age of the sample did not moderate the association, Q(114) = 3.67, p > .05. The percentage of males in the sample also did not moderate the relation between fear-avoidance and pain intensity, Q(115) = 1.14, p > .05.

Hofstede’s cultural dimensions were examined as continuous moderators using meta-regression. One study was excluded from these analyses because it recruited participants from four different countries. The results of these analyses indicated three of Hofstede’s dimensions were significant moderators: Power Distance Index, Individualism versus Collectivism, and Indulgence versus Restraint. The Power Distance Index moderated the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity, indicating that as ratings on the PDI increased (indicating increased comfort with power inequalities), the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity was stronger (Q(116) = 7.06, p < .05). The Individualism versus Collectivism scale was also a significant moderator of this association, such that as ratings on this scale decreased (less individualist or more collectivist ratings), the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity was stronger (Q(116) = 15.15, p < .001). Finally, the Indulgence versus Restraint scale moderated the association, indicating that as scores on this scale decreased (indulgence decreases), the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity was stronger (Q(115) = 6.95, p < .05).

Finally, the measurement characteristics of the studies were considered as moderators (see Table 5). The first analysis examined whether the use of one or multiple measures of fear-avoidance moderated the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. This moderation was significant, Q(1) = 5.60, p <.05. The results indicated that for studies using multiple measures of fear-avoidance, the weighted effect size was significantly higher (r(45) = .30) than for studies using a single measure (r(73) = .24). Furthermore, a moderation analysis evaluated whether the use of pain-specific fear-avoidance measures, general avoidance measures, or both types of measures affected the strength of the association. The majority of studies (k = 99) utilized only pain-specific fear-avoidance measures. Analyses indicated that the type of fear-avoidance measure significantly moderated the association (Q(2) = 15.91, p <.05). Studies that utilized pain-specific measures of fear-avoidance had a higher weighted effect size (r(99) = .27) than those that used measures of general fear-avoidance (r(6) = .02). Studies that utilized both pain-specific and general fear-avoidance had a small-to-moderate weighted effect size (r(13) = .30). To further explore this moderation, effect sizes were calculated using the average of only pain-specific measures within a study as well as effect sizes using only general fear-avoidance measures. The weighted effect sizes are reported in Table 3.

Table 5

Tests of meta-ANOVA for moderation of association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity.

Moderator Point estimate 95% Cl z k Q
Total .26[***] .23−.29 15.00[***] 112 691.42[***]
 Pain measure 3.58
 One-item pain .24 .21−.27 14.24[***] 63
 Multiple item pain .30 .24−.35 10.61[***] 54
Sample 3.89
 Chronic pain .27 .23−.32 10.88[***] 66
 Chronic back pain .24 .21−.28 13.52[***] 30
 Fibromyalgia .32 .09−.51 2.68[**] 4
 Chronic spinal pain .30 .25−.35 10.17[***] 3
 Chronic headache .24 .09−.38 3.03[**] 4
Number of fear-avoidance measures 5.60[*]
 One measure .24 .19−.28 10.40[***] 73
 Multiple measures .30 .27−.33 18.93[***] 45
Type of fear-avoidance measure 15.91[*]
 Pain-specific fear-avoidance .27 .24−.31 14.49[***] 99
 General fear-avoidance .02 −.12to .15 .21 6
 Pain-specific and general fear-avoidance .30 .25 to .35 12.14[***] 13
  1. Note. Values in the Q column indicate heterogeneity in a variable.

    All analysis used a mixed effects approach.

4.2 Publication bias

Trim-and-fill analyses indicated no evidence of publication bias. As shown in Fig. 2, the funnel plot appears to be relatively symmetrical, with no imputed studies to the left of the mean. This provides no evidence of publication bias.

A histogram of p-values for studies with significant correlations is shown in Fig. 3. Over 80% of the statistically significant effects included in the analyses were significant at p < .01. p-curve analyses indicated that the studies did not suggest evidence of p-hacking (p > .999). The p-curve graph is shown in Fig. 4. The p-curve analyses suggest that there is no evidence of a bias toward reporting only significant effects (Tables 4 and 5).

Fig. 3 
							Histogram representing the frequency of p-values for all statistically significant effects.
Fig. 3

Histogram representing the frequency of p-values for all statistically significant effects.

Fig. 4 
							P-curve depicting distribution of p values forall statistically significant effects (p < .05).
Fig. 4

P-curve depicting distribution of p values forall statistically significant effects (p < .05).

5 Discussion

This is the first meta-analysis to compute an estimate of the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity in chronic pain patients. The results indicate that fear-avoidance and pain intensity are positively associated, and this link is of small-to-moderate magnitude. This finding suggests that those with higher pain intensity may have a greater propensity to avoid activities that worsen pain, or those who are more avoidant report higher pain intensity. Importantly, the magnitude of the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity varied as a function of several moderating variables. Specifically, the association between pain intensity and fear-avoidance varied as a function of cultural characteristics (using Hofstede’s dimensions) and characteristics of the measures employed in the studies. The association did not vary in strength based on type of sample, pain duration, age, gender, or marital status.

The association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity varied significantly as a function of Hofstede’s cultural dimensions. The results indicated that the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity was stronger in cultures where individuals are comfortable with an unequal distribution of power in society, in cultures where collectivist ideals are valued, and in cultures where restraint is more important than short-term gratification. These findings indicate that cross-cultural comparisons within the chronic pain literature are critically important. For example, previous research has explored the role of health care providers’ beliefs about the extent to which chronic pain has a psychological or physical cause, finding that treatment of chronic pain as a strictly physical condition adds stress to chronic pain patients [40]. A cultural and medical focus on autonomy isolates the individual from their environment, which focuses on the individual’s role in the disease process, rather than lifestyle changes or preventive factors that might improve quality of life [41]. The current findings suggest that cultural norms and beliefs may impact the extent to which pain intensity is associated with fear-avoidance. It is hypothesized that in cultures where collectivism and restraint are valued, avoidance may serve a broader function. When in pain, it may be more culturally appropriate to avoid pain, as the pain may draw attention to the individual or indicate a lack of restraint. However, this is a preliminary hypothesis, and future research should explore the sociological and cultural influences that affect fear-avoidance and pain intensity in chronic pain patients to better understand the impact of societal variations in values and normative behavior. In addition, Hofstede’s cultural dimensions are applied at a national level, but many cultures exist within nations, and other concerns about the validity of these dimensions have been raised [27]. For this reason, future research should examine culture at an individual level to increase the specific dimensions of culture’s role in the association between pain intensity and fear-avoidance.

The results indicated that studies using multiple measures of fear-avoidance had a higher weighted mean effect size than studies that used one measure of fear-avoidance. This finding could be due to increased precision of measurement that comes with more items to measure a similar construct [42].

An additional measurement-related finding was that studies utilizing pain-specific measures or both pain-specific and general measures had a higher weighted effect size than those using only general fear-avoidance measures. Interpretation of this finding is limited by the fact that there were few k = 6) studies that solely utilized general fear-avoidance measures. The analyses indicated that the correlation between general fear-avoidance and pain intensity is of lesser magnitude than the correlation between pain-specific fear-avoidance and pain intensity. Pain-specific stimuli may be inherently more salient with chronic pain patients, so measures that target pain-specific fear-avoidance may resonate more with this population. In sum, general measures of fear and avoidance should only be used in conjunction with pain-specific measures–never alone.

The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain is a well-established conceptualization of the factors that impact the development and maintenance of chronic pain, as well as the possible consequences of fear and avoidance. The current analyses indicate a small-tomoderate association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. Those with higher reported pain intensity may be more prone to avoid pain. Alternatively, individuals who report more fear-avoidance may also experience increased pain intensity. The avoidance of pain, though successful in the short-term, may increase pain intensity by physical deconditioning or by reinforcing the notion that pain should be avoided. Future research should explore the mechanisms by which fear-avoidance and pain intensity are associated using mediation analyses and/or prospective study designs.

Despite the significant body of research supporting the fear-avoidance model of chronic pain, it remains a problematic construct. The term fear-avoidance suggests that fear and avoidance co-occur simultaneously. These two processes can certainly occur independently because an individual can experience fear and choose to approach a stimulus, or an individual can avoid a stimulus but not self-report fear. This conceptual issue also has methodological significance. Many of the fear-avoidance measures included in the current meta-analysis confound fear and avoidance, which prevents the separation of these two constructs. This not only confounds the measurement instruments, but also introduces error into the correlation between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. Future research should focus on the development of instruments that separate fear and avoidance to better understand the differential predictive value of these variables.

The current study represents the first meta-analysis to estimate the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. However, several limitations should be considered. First, only cross-sectional data were considered in the analyses to avoid including confounding variables with the effects of interventions. Thus, no causal inferences can be drawn from the results. Second, many studies did not collect or report adequate information to be included in all moderation analyses, limiting the conclusions that can be drawn. Third, the majority of the included studies did not report reliability coefficients for the sample; thus, corrections for artifacts in each sample could not be applied. As a result, the correlations may include error that is specific to the measure and the sample. Fourth, though the International Association for the Study of Pain has indicated that three or more months qualifies as chronic pain, the inclusion criteria across studies varied considerably [43]. Therefore, the test of pain duration as a moderator was limited by the restricted range of average pain duration in the included studies. The lowest reported mean pain duration included was 12 months, which limits the ability of these results to generalize to all chronic pain patients. Some moderation analyses were not conducted because not enough studies reported data (e.g., attention to pain, negative affect). Future research should explore other variables that may affect the strength of the association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity in chronic pain patients. Fifth, several acceptance measures were included in the current analyses. Most commonly, the CPAQ [24] was included, as it was originally developed [44] based on the AAQ [18], a measure of experiential avoidance. The AAQ has been used as an indicator of acceptance when reverse-scored [22, 23]. The inclusion of both acceptance and avoidance measures in the current analyses is a limitation that is reflective of measurement imprecision in the broader literature. Nevertheless, these measures were included in an effort to provide the most comprehensive picture of the literature. Finally, the use of Hofstede’s dimensions of national culture have been criticized for lacking validity [27]. As such, the current findings should be interpreted with caution, and more idiographic measures should be utilized in future research.

This study has significant implications for future research. The directionality of the relation between fear-avoidance and pain intensity needs to be further explored. If fear-avoidance is a risk factor for increased pain intensity, then measures of fear-avoidance should be utilized in the identification of individuals who would benefit from behavioral interventions focused on engaging in approach behaviors. If higher pain intensity places an individual at risk to fear pain and avoid activities that could increase pain, then those that report high pain intensity should be targeted for interventions in which movement and exposure to pain is emphasized. Furthermore, in those with higher pain intensity, acceptance may be a central component of intervention work. Although the intensity of pain may dissuade an individual from movement, the extent to which this limits an individual from engaging in a meaningful life is critical. In addition, gaining a better understanding of cultural differences in coping with chronic pain is an essential next step. Health behaviors surrounding both acute and chronic pain must be considered, along with health care policies and beliefs within different countries and cultures.

In summary, the results of the present meta-analysis indicated a small-to-moderate positive association between fear-avoidance and pain intensity. The results were robust to demographic and pain characteristics.These findings have significant implications for behavioral treatments in chronic pain populations. In-vivo exposure [45] and cognitive-behavioral therapy have shown success in improving psychological and physical functioning among chronic pain patients [46]. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, a third-wave cognitive-behavioral therapy, has also improved quality of life and functioning in a variety of chronic pain samples [47]. Through these treatments, avoidance is targeted as the key process of change through exposure to feared stimuli and promotion of approach behavior. Future research should focus on understanding the demographic characteristics, pain characteristics, and other biopsychosocial variables that affect the success of behavioral treatments in improving quality of life and functioning among chronic pain patients. This will allow for tailored treatments, with the hope of optimizing treatment outcomes for those experiencing chronic pain.

Highlights

  • Across studies, fear-avoidance and pain intensity were positively associated.

  • Cultural differences were a significant moderator of the association.

  • Findings can inform intervention and prevention work with chronic pain patients.


DOI of refers to article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.08.010.


  1. Ethical issues: Informed consent was not required, and approval by an ethics board was not obtained, as only aggregated secondary data was examined. Study protocol is not registered.

  2. Conflict of interest: No conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Dr. Michael O’Hara, Dr. Kristian Markon, Dr. James Marchman, Cara Solness, Dr. Teresa Treat, Dr. Ernest O’Boyle, and Dr. Mark Vander Weg for the helpful feedback on this manuscript.

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

[1] Boris-Karpel S. Policy and practice issues in pain management. In: Ebert MH, Kerns RD, editors. Behavioral and psychopharmacologic pain management. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press; 2010., http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511781445.025.Search in Google Scholar

[2] Gatchel RJ, Peng YB, Peters ML, Fuchs PN, Turk DC. The biopsychosocial approach to chronic pain: scientific advances and future directions. Psychol Bull 2007;133:581–624, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.581.Search in Google Scholar

[3] Kerns RD, Sellinger J, Goodin BR. Psychological treatment of chronic pain. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2011;7:411–34, http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-090310-120430.Search in Google Scholar

[4] Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain 2000;85:317–32, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0.Search in Google Scholar

[5] *Asmundson GJ, Norton PJ, Veloso F. Anxiety sensitivity and fear of pain in patients with recurring headaches. Behav Res Ther 1999;37:703–13, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00172-7.Search in Google Scholar

[6] Asmundson GJG, Norton PJ, Norton GR. Beyond pain: the role of fear and avoidance in chronicity. Clin Psychol Rev 1999;19:97–119, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00034-8.Search in Google Scholar

[7] Bortz WM. The disuse syndrome. West J Med 1984;141:691–4.Search in Google Scholar

[8] *McCracken LM, Gross RT, Aikens J, Carnrike CLM. The assessment of anxiety and fear in persons with chronic pain: a comparison of instruments. Behav Res Ther 1996;34:927–33, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00057-5.Search in Google Scholar

[9] Zale EL, Lange KL, Fields SA, Ditre JW. The relation between pain-related fear and disability: a meta-analysis. J Pain 2013;14:1019–30, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2013.05.005.Search in Google Scholar

[10] Gheldof EL, Vinck J, Van den Bussche E, Vlaeyen JW, Hidding A, Crombez G. Pain and pain-related fear are associated with functional and social disability in an occupational setting: evidence of mediation by pain-related fear. Eur J Pain 2006;10:513–25, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.07.005.Search in Google Scholar

[11] Mannion AF, Junge A, Taimela S, Müntener M, Lorenzo K, Dvorak J. Active therapy for chronic low back pain: part 3. Factors influencing self-rated disability and its change following therapy. Spine 2001;26:920–9.Search in Google Scholar

[12] Sorbi MJ, Peters ML, Kruise DA, Maas CJ, Kerssens JJ, Verhaak PF, Bensing JM. Electronic momentary assessment in chronic pain II: pain and psychological pain responses as predictors of pain disability. The Clin J Pain 2006;22:67–81, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.ajp.0000148625.84874.48.Search in Google Scholar

[13] Crombez G, Eccleston C, Van Damme S, Vlaeyen JW, Karoly P. Fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: the next generation. Clin J Pain 2012;28:475–83.Search in Google Scholar

[14] Swinkels-Meewisse IE, Roelofs J, Schouten EG, Verbeek AL, Oostendoorp RA, Vlaeyen JW. Fear of movement/(re)injury predicting chronic disabling low back pain: a prospective inception cohort study. SPINE 2006;31:658–64, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.brs.0000203709.65384.9d.Search in Google Scholar

[15] *Dehghani M, Sharpe L, Nicholas MK. Structural evaluation of the contemporary psychological models of chronic pain: does fear of pain work for all? Cognit Behav Ther 2010;39:214–24, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/16506070903390134.Search in Google Scholar

[16] *Ramírez -Maestre C, Esteve R, Lopez-Martinez A. Fear-avoidance, pain acceptance and adjustment to chronic pain: a cross-sectional study on a sample of 686 patients with chronic spinal pain. Ann Behav Med 2014;48:402–10, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9619-6.Search in Google Scholar

[17] Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med 2009;6:e1000097, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed1000097.Search in Google Scholar

[18] Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG, Bissett RT, Pistorello J, Toarmino D, Polusny MA, Dykstra TA, Batten SV, Bergan J, Stewart SH, Zvolensky MJ, Eifert GH, Bond FW, Forsyth JP, Karekla M, McCurry SM. Measuring experiential avoidance: a preliminary test of a working model. Psychol Rec 2004;54:553–78.Search in Google Scholar

[19] Waddell G, Newton M, Henderson I, Somerville D, Main CJ. A fear-avoidance beliefs questionnaire (FABQ) and the role of fear-avoidance beliefs in chronic low back pain and disability. Pain 1993;52:157–68, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(93)90127-B.Search in Google Scholar

[20] Chawla N, Ostafin B. Experiential avoidance as a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology: an empirical review. J Clin Psychol 2007;63:871–90, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20400.Search in Google Scholar

[21] Costa J, Pinto-Gouveia J. The mediation effect of experiential avoidance between coping and psychopathology in chronic pain. Clin Psychol Psychother 2011;18:34–47, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.699.Search in Google Scholar

[22] *McCracken LM, Velleman SC. Psychological flexibility in adults with chronic pain: a study of acceptance, mindfulness, and values-based action in primary care. Pain 2010;148:141–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.034.Search in Google Scholar

[23] *McCracken LM, Zhao-O’Brien J. General psychological acceptance and chronic pain: there is more to accept than the pain itself. Eur J Pain 2010;14:170–5, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.03.004.Search in Google Scholar

[24] *McCracken LM, Vowles KE, Eccleston C. Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain 2004;107:159–66, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.10.012.Search in Google Scholar

[25] Hofstede GH. Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. 2nd ed. Sage Publications; 2001.Search in Google Scholar

[26] Sondergaard M. Hofstede’s consequences: a study of reviews, citations and replications. Org Stud 1994;15:447–56, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/017084069401500307.Search in Google Scholar

[27] McSweeney B. Hofstede’s model of national cultural differences and their consequences: a triumph of faith—a failure of analysis. Hum Relat 2002;55:89–118, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0018726702055001602.Search in Google Scholar

[28] Fleiss JL, Levin B, Paik MC. The measurement of interrater agreement. In: Statistical methods for rates and proportions. 3rd ed. Wiley; 2003, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/0471445428.ch18.Search in Google Scholar

[29] Lipsey MA, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage publications; 2001.Search in Google Scholar

[30] Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Version 2) [Computer software]. Englewood, NJ: Biostat; 2014. Available from http://www.meta-analysis.com.Search in Google Scholar

[31] Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000;56:455–63, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x.Search in Google Scholar

[32] Sutton AJ. Publication bias. In: Harris Cooper, Larry V. Hedges, Jeffrey C. Valentine, editors. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. New York: Russell Sage Foundation; 2009.Search in Google Scholar

[33] Lau J, Ioannidis J, Terrin N, Schmid CH, Olkin I. The case of the mis-leading funnel plot. Br Med J 2006;333:597–600, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.333.7568.597.Search in Google Scholar

[34] Peters JL, Sutton AJ, Jones DR, Abrams KR, Rushton L. Performance of the trim and fill method in the presence of publication bias and between-study heterogeneity. Stat Med 2007;26:4544–62, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.2889.Search in Google Scholar

[35] Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, Simmons JP. P-curve: a key to the file drawer. J Exp Psychol: Gen 2013;143:534–47, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033242.Search in Google Scholar

[36] Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, Simmons JP. P-curve and effect size: correcting for publication bias using only significant results. Perspect Psychol Sci 2014;9:666–81, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691614553988.Search in Google Scholar

[37] Field AP. Is the meta-analysis of correlation coefficients accurate when population correlations vary? Psychol Methods 2005;10:444, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.444.Search in Google Scholar

[38] Hedges LV, Vevea JL. Fixed-and random-effects models in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods 1998;3:486–504, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.3.4.486.Search in Google Scholar

[39] Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 2002;21:1539–58, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.1186.Search in Google Scholar

[40] Holzman AD, Turk DC. Pain management: a handbook of psychological treatment approaches. New York: Pergamon Press; 1986.Search in Google Scholar

[41] Bates MS. A critical perspective on coronary artery disease and coronary bypass surgery. Soc Sci Med 1990;30:249–60, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0277-9536(90)90086-8.Search in Google Scholar

[42] Churchill GA, Peter JP. Research design effects of the reliability of rating scales: a meta-analysis. J Market Res 1994;21:360–75, http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3151463.Search in Google Scholar

[43] Merskey H, Bogduk N, editors. Classification of chronic pain: descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Seattle, USA: International Association for the Study Pain; 1994.Search in Google Scholar

[44] *Geiser DS [Doctoral dissertation] A comparison of acceptance-focused and control-focused psychological treatments in a chronic pain treatment center; 1992. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9315546).Search in Google Scholar

[45] *de Jong JR, Vlaeyen JW, Onghena P, Goossens ME, Geilen M, Mulder H. Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain: education or exposure in vivo as mediator to fear reduction? Clin J Pain 2005;21:9–17, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00002508-200501000-00002.Search in Google Scholar

[46] Morley S, Eccleston C, Williams A. Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive behaviour therapy and behaviour therapy for chronic pain in adults, excluding headache. Pain 1999;80:1–3, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00255-3.Search in Google Scholar

[47] Vowles KE, Thompson M. Acceptance and commitment therapy for chronic pain. In: McCracken L, editor. Mindfulness and acceptance in behavioral medicine: current theory and practice. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc.; 2011.Search in Google Scholar

[48] *Asghari A, Nicholas MK. Pain self-efficacy beliefs and pain behaviour. A prospective study. Pain 2001;94:85–100, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00344-X.Search in Google Scholar

[49] *Bendayan R, Esteve R, Blanca MJ. New empirical evidence of the validity of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire: the differential influence of activity engagement and pain willingness on adjustment to chronic pain. Br J Health Psychol 2012;17:314–26, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8287.2011.02039.x.Search in Google Scholar

[50] *Boersma K, Linton SJ. Expectancy, fear and pain in the prediction of chronic pain and disability: a prospective analysis. Eur J Pain 2006;10:551–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.08.004.Search in Google Scholar

[51] *Callahan CD [Doctoral dissertation] Stress, coping, and personality hardiness in the etiology of temporomandibular disorders; 1991. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9129541).Search in Google Scholar

[52] *Cannon CK [Doctoral dissertation] Depressive symptoms in patients with chronic pain: delineation and empirical investigation of cognitive-behavioural mediation models; 1999. Retrieved from ProQuest. (NQ35954).Search in Google Scholar

[53] *Carleton RN [Doctoral dissertation] Chronic pain and startle; 2005. Retrieved from ProQuest. (MR05990).Search in Google Scholar

[54] *Carleton RN, Abrams MP, Kachur SS, Asmundson GJ. Waddell’s symptoms as correlates of vulnerabilities associated with fear-anxiety—avoidance models of pain: pain-related anxiety, catastrophic thinking, perceived disability, and treatment outcome. J Occup Rehabil 2009;19:364–74, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10926-009-9191-2.Search in Google Scholar

[55] *Carleton RN, Richter AA, Asmundson GJ. Attention modification in persons with fibromyalgia: a double blind, randomized clinical trial. Cognit Behav Ther 2011;40:279–90, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2011.616218.Search in Google Scholar

[56] *Carranza LV [Doctoral dissertation] The role of anxiety sensitivity in pain chronicity: tension headache sufferers in a college population; 2002. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3020930).Search in Google Scholar

[57] *Cascarilla EA [Doctoral dissertation] Chronic pain-related distress & disability: an empirical investigation of a modern behavioral theory of acceptance of chronic pain; 2010. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3393044).Search in Google Scholar

[58] *Chiu H [Doctoral dissertation] An analysis of outcomes of a pain management program; 2002. Retrieved from ProQuest. (1407897).Search in Google Scholar

[59] *Cho S, Heiby EM, McCracken LM, Lee SM, Moon DE. Pain-related anxiety as a mediator of the effects of mindfulness on physical and psychosocial functioning in chronic pain patients in Korea. J Pain 2010;11:789–97, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2009.12.006.Search in Google Scholar

[60] *Cho SK, Heiby EM, McCracken LM, Moon DE, Lee JH. Daily functioning in chronic pain: study of structural relations with posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms, pain intensity, and pain avoidance. Kor J Pain 2011;24:13–21, http://dx.doi.org/10.3344/kjp.2011.24.1.13.Search in Google Scholar

[61] *Cook AJ, Brawer PA, Vowles KE. The fear-avoidance model of chronic pain: validation and age analysis using structural equation modeling. Pain 2006;121:195–206, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2005.11.018.Search in Google Scholar

[62] *Crombez G, Eccleston C, Baeyens F, van Houdenhove B, Van Den Broecks. Attention to chronic pain is dependent upon pain-related fear. J Psychosom Res 1999;47:403–10, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00046-X.Search in Google Scholar

[63] *Crombez G, Vlaeyen JWS, Heuts PHTG, Lysens R. Pain-related fear is more disabling then pain itself: evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain 1999;80:329–39, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00229-2.Search in Google Scholar

[64] *Crowley D, Kendall NA. Development and initial validation of a questionnaire for measuring fear-avoidance associated with pain: the fear-avoidance of pain scale. J Musculoskel Pain 1999;7:3–19, http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J094v07n03_02.Search in Google Scholar

[65] *Cui J, Matsushima E, Aso K, Masuda A, Makita K. Psychological features and coping styles in patients with chronic pain. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2009;63:147–52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.01934.x.Search in Google Scholar

[66] Dehghani M, Sharpe L, Nicholas MK. Selective attention to pain-related information in chronic musculoskeletal pain patients. Pain 2003;105:37–46, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00224-0.Search in Google Scholar

[67] *Denison E, Asenlof P, Lindberg P. Self-efficacy, fear avoidance, and pain intensity as predictors of disability in subacute and chronic musculoskeletal pain patients in primary health care. Pain 2004;111:245–52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.001.Search in Google Scholar

[68] *Dyson SJ [Doctoral dissertation] Predicting outcomes in chronic low back pain rehabilitation: roles of pain-related anxiety and physical activity; 2014. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3591066).Search in Google Scholar

[69] *Endler NS, Corace KM, Summerfeldt LJ, Johnson JM, Rothbart P. Coping with chronic pain. Personal Individ Differ 2003;34:323–46, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00048-X.Search in Google Scholar

[70] *Esteve R, Ramirez-Maestre C. Pain fear avoidance and pain acceptance: a cross-sectional study comparing their influence on adjustment to chronic pain across three samples of patients. Ann Behav Med 2013;46:169–80, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9499-1.Search in Google Scholar

[71] *Esteve R, Ramirez-Maestre C, Lopez-Martinez AE. Experiential avoidance and anxiety sensitivity as dispositional variables and their relationship to the adjustment to chronic pain. Eur J Pain 2012;16:718–26, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2011.00035.x.Search in Google Scholar

[72] *Farin E, Gramm L, Schmidt E. The patient-physician relationship in patients with chronic low back pain as a predictor of outcomes after rehabilitation. J Behav Med 2013;36:246–58, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-012-9419-z.Search in Google Scholar

[73] *Fersum KV, O’Sullivan P, Skouen JS, Smith A, Kvale A. Efficacy of classification-based cognitive functional therapy in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain: a randomized controlled trial. Eur J Pain 2013;17:916–28, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00252.x.Search in Google Scholar

[74] *Fish RA, McGuire B, Hogan M, Morrison TG, Stewart I. Validation of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ) in an internet sample and development and preliminary validation of the CPAQ-8. Pain 2010;149:435–43, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.12.016.Search in Google Scholar

[75] *Foote HL [Doctoral dissertation] Psychological flexibility in migraine patients: the role of acceptance and values based action; 2013. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3612746).Search in Google Scholar

[76] *French DJ, France CR, Vigneau F, French JA, Evans RT. Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic pain: a psychometric assessment of the original English version of the Tampa scale for kinesiophobia (TSK). Pain 2007;127:42–51, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.07.016.Search in Google Scholar

[77] *Gomez-Perez L, Lopez-Martinez AE, Ruiz-Parraga GT. Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Tampa scale for kinesiophobia (TSK). J Pain 2011;12:425–35, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2010.08.004.Search in Google Scholar

[78] *Goubert L, Crombez G, Van Damme S. The role of neuroticism, pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in vigilance to pain: a structural equations approach. Pain 2004;107:234–41, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.11.005.Search in Google Scholar

[79] *Goubert L, Francken G, Crombez G, Vansteenwegen D, Lysens R. Exposure to physical movement in chronic back pain patients: no evidence for generalization across different movements. Behav Res Ther 2002;40:415–29, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00020-1.Search in Google Scholar

[80] *Grotle M, Vollestad NK, Veierod MB, Brox JI. Fear-avoidance beliefs and distress in relation to disability in acute and chronic low back pain. Pain 2004;112:343–52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.09.020.Search in Google Scholar

[81] *Jacox CM [Doctoral dissertation] Coping styles and psychological distress in women with endometriosis; 1995. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9608293).Search in Google Scholar

[82] *Jeffrey JE [Doctoral dissertation] Predictors of quality of life for adults who have rheumatoid arthritis; 1989. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9013022).Search in Google Scholar

[83] *Kindermans HP, Roelofs J, Goossens ME, Huijen IP, Verbunt JA, Vlaeyen JW. Activity patterns in chronic pain: underlying dimensions and associations with disability and depressed mood. J Pain 2011;12:1049–58, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2011.04.009.Search in Google Scholar

[84] *Knaster P, Estlander AM, Karlsson H, Kaprio J, Kalso E. Temperament traits and chronic pain: the association of harm avoidance and pain-related anxiety. PLoS ONE 2012;7:e45672, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045672.Search in Google Scholar

[85] *Koho P, Aho S, Watson P, Hurri H. Assessment of chronic pain behaviour: reliability of the method and its relationship with perceived disability, physical impairment and function. J Rehab Med 2001;33:128–32, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/165019701750165970.Search in Google Scholar

[86] *Lamoth CJC, Meijer OG, Daffertshofer A, Wuisman PIJM, Beek PJ. Effects of chronic low back pain on trunk coordination and back muscle activity during walking: changes in motor control. Eur Spine J 2006;15:23–40, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00586-004-0825-y.Search in Google Scholar

[87] *Larsen DK, Taylor S, Asmundson GJ. Exploratory factor analysis of the Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale in patients with chronic pain complaints. Pain 1997;69:27–34, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03232-0.Search in Google Scholar

[88] *Larsson C, Hansson EE, Sundquist K, Jakobsson U. Psychometric properties of the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia (TSK-11) among older people with chronic pain. Physiother Theory Pract 2014:1–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09593985.2013.877546.Search in Google Scholar

[89] *Laufer Y, Elheiga-Na’amne BA, Rozen N. Translation and validation of the Arab version of the fear-avoidance beliefs questionnaire. J Back Musculoskelet Rehabil 2012;25:201–8.Search in Google Scholar

[90] *Lee K [Doctoral dissertation] To investigate the effects of the fear-avoidance beliefs on Chinese patients with neck pain; 2008. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3282303).Search in Google Scholar

[91] *Lopez-Martinez AE, Ramirez-Maestre C, Esteve R. An examination of the structural link between post-traumatic stress symptoms and chronic pain in the framework of fear-avoidance models. Eur J Pain 2014;18:1129–38, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.00459.x.Search in Google Scholar

[92] *Luedtke K, Rushton A, Wright C, Juergens TP, Mueller G, May A. Effectiveness of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with chronic low back pain: design, method and protocol for a randomised controlled trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 2011;12:290, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-12-290.Search in Google Scholar

[93] *Lundberg M, Frennered K, Hagg O, Styf J. The impact of fear-avoidance model variables on disability in patients with specific or nonspecific chronic low back pain. Spine 2011;36:1547–53, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181f61660.Search in Google Scholar

[94] *Macleod LS [Doctoral dissertation] Baseline approach/withdrawal motivation as a diathesis for the development of dysfunction in chronic temporomandibular disorder pain; 2008. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3280878).Search in Google Scholar

[95] *Martin AL, Halket E, Asmundson GJ, Flora DB, Katz J. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and the diathesis-stress model of chronic pain and disability in patients undergoing major surgery. Clin J Pain 2010;26:518–27, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e3181e15b98.Search in Google Scholar

[96] *Martin AL, McGrath PA, Brown SC, Katz J. Anxiety sensitivity, fear of pain and pain-related disability in children and adolescents with chronic pain. Pain Res Manage 2007;12:267–72.Search in Google Scholar

[97] *McCracken LM. Acceptance of chronic pain: component analysis and a revised assessment method. Pain 2004;107:159–66, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2003.10.012.Search in Google Scholar

[98] *McCracken LM, Gauntlett-Gilbert J, Eccleston C. Acceptance of pain in adolescents with chronic pain: validation of an adapted assessment instrument and preliminary correlation analyses. Eur J Pain 2010;14:316–20, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2009.05.002.Search in Google Scholar

[99] *McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Coping or acceptance: what to do about chronic pain? Pain 2003;105:197–204, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00202-1.Search in Google Scholar

[100] *McCracken LM, Keogh E. Acceptance, mindfulness, and values-based action may counteract fear and avoidance of emotions in chronic pain: an analysis of anxiety sensitivity. J Pain 2009;10:408–15, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2008.09.015.Search in Google Scholar

[101] *McCracken LM, Gauntlett-Gilbert J, Vowles KE. The role of mind-fulness in a contextual cognitive-behavioral analysis of chronic pain-related suffering and disability. Pain 2007;131:63–9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.12.013.Search in Google Scholar

[102] *McCracken LM, Spertus IL, Janeck AS, Sinclair D, Wetzel FT. Behavioral dimensions of adjustment in persons with chronic pain: pain-related anxiety and acceptance. Pain 1999;80:283–9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00219-X.Search in Google Scholar

[103] *Meyer JA [Doctoral dissertation] Integration of the avoidance cycle with the schema enmeshment model of pain: relationships with quality of life and disability in chronic, nonmalignant pain; 2010. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3381939).Search in Google Scholar

[104] *Meyer K, Sprott H, Mannion AF. Cross-cultural adaption, reliability, and validity of the German version of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. J Psychosom Res 2008;64:469–78, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.12.004.Search in Google Scholar

[105] *Michalski D, Liebig S, Thomae E, Hinz A, Bergh FT. Pain in patients with multiple sclerosis: a complex assessment including quantitative and qualitative measurements provides for a disease-related biopsychosocial pain model. J Pain Res 2011;4:219–25, http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/JPR.S20309.Search in Google Scholar

[106] *Newton-John TR, Mason C, Hunter M. The role of resilience in adjustment and coping with chronic pain. Rehabil Psychol 2014;59:360–5, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037023.Search in Google Scholar

[107] *Nicholas MK, Asghari A. Investigating acceptance in adjustment to chronic pain: is acceptance broader than we thought? Pain 2006;124:269–79, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.032.Search in Google Scholar

[108] *Nicholas MK, Asghari A, Corbett M, Smeets RJ, Wood BM, Overton S, Perry C, Tonkin LE, Beeston L. Is adherence to pain self-management strategies associated with improved pain, depression and disability in those with disabling chronic pain? Eur J Pain 2012;16:93–104, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.005.Search in Google Scholar

[109] *O’Connell MJ [Doctoral dissertation] The relationship between coping and adjustment: a multidimensional investigation of the chronic pain condition; 1995. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9432625).Search in Google Scholar

[110] *Ojala T, Piirainen A, Sipilla K, Suutama T, Hakkinen A. Reliability and validity of the Finnish version of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ). Disabil Rehabil 2013;35:306–14, http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/09638288.2012.694572.Search in Google Scholar

[111] *Perry EV, Francis AJ. Self-efficacy, pain-related fear, and disability in a heterogeneous pain sample. Pain Manage Nurs 2013;14:e124–34, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pmn.2011.09.001.Search in Google Scholar

[112] *Plumb Vilardaga JC [Doctoral dissertation] Acceptance and commitment therapy for longstanding chronic pain in a community-based outpatient group setting; 2013. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3550275).Search in Google Scholar

[113] *Ramírez-Maestre C, Esteve R. The role of sex/gender in the experience of pain: resilience, fear, and acceptance as central variables in the adjustment of men and women with chronic pain. J Pain 2014;15:608–18, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.02.006.Search in Google Scholar

[114] *Riecke J, Holzapfel S, Rief W, Glombiewski JA. Evaluation and implementation of graded in vivo exposure for chronic low back pain in a German outpatient setting: a study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2013;14:203, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1745-6215-14-203.Search in Google Scholar

[115] *Rodero B, Garcia-Campayo J, Casanueva B, Lopez del Hoyo Y, Serrano-Blanco A, Luciano JV. Validation of the Spanish version of the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ) for the assessment of acceptance in fibromyalgia. Health Q Life Outcomes 2010;8:37, http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-8-37.Search in Google Scholar

[116] *Samwel HJ, Kraaimaat FW, Crul BJ, Evers AW. The role of fear-avoidance and helplessness in explaining functional disability in chronic pain: a prospective study. Int J Behav Med 2007;14:237–41, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03002998.Search in Google Scholar

[117] *Samwel HJA, Evers AWM, Crul BJP, Kraaimaat FW. The role of helplessness, fear of pain, and passive pain-coping in chronic pain patients. Clin J Pain 2006;22:245–51, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.ajp.0000173019.72365.f5.Search in Google Scholar

[118] *Schutze R, Rees C, Preece M, Schutze M. Low mindfulness predicts pain catastrophizing in a fear-avoidance model of chronic pain. Pain 2010;148:120–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.030.Search in Google Scholar

[119] *Sil S, Thomas S, DiCesare C, Strotman D, Ting TV, Myer G, Kashikar-Zuck S. Preliminary evidence of altered biomechanics in adolescents with Juvenile Fibromyalgia. Arthrit Care Res 2014, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acr.22450.Search in Google Scholar

[120] *Simons LE, Kaczynski KJ. The Fear Avoidance model of chronic pain: examination for pediatric application. J Pain 2012;13:827–35, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.05.002.Search in Google Scholar

[121] *Simons LE, Pielech M, Capucci S, Lebel A. Fear of pain in pediatric headache. Cephalalgia 2014;35:36–44, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0333102414534084.Search in Google Scholar

[122] *Strahl C, Kleinknecht RA, Dinnel DL. The role of pain anxiety, coping, and pain self-efficacy in rheumatoid arthritis patient functioning. Behav Res Ther 2000;38:863–73, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00102-3.Search in Google Scholar

[123] *Sullivan MJ, Adams H, Horan S, Maher D, Boland D, Gross R. The role of perceived injustice in the experience of chronic pain and disability: scale development and validation. J Occup Rehabil 2008;18:249–61, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10926-008-9140-5.Search in Google Scholar

[124] *Suttenfield CW [Doctoral dissertation] Fear of pain: a cognitive model for the development and maintenance of chronic pain behavior; 1995. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9537714).Search in Google Scholar

[125] *Thomas AM [Doctoral dissertation] An exploratory correlational study of chronic pain acceptance and trauma symptomatology in persons involved in Internet-based chronic pain support groups; 2012. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3458178).Search in Google Scholar

[126] *Tkachuk GA, Harris CA. Psychometric properties of the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia-11 (TSK-11). J Pain 2012;13:970–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.07.001.Search in Google Scholar

[127] *Trompetter HR, Bohlmeijer ET, van Baalen B, Kleen M, Koke A, Reneman M, Karlein MG. The Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS): exploration of psychometric properties in a heterogeneous chronic pain sample. Eur J Psychol Assess 2014, http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000191.Search in Google Scholar

[128] *Trost Z, France CR, Thomas JS. Exposure to movement in chronic back pain: evidence of successful generalization across a reaching task. Pain 2008;137:26–33, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.08.005.Search in Google Scholar

[129] *Turk DC, Robinson JP, Burwinkle T. Prevalence of fear of pain and activity in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. J Pain 2004;5:483–90, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2004.08.002.Search in Google Scholar

[130] *Viane I, Crombez G, Eccleston C, Poppe C, Devulder J, Van Houdenhove B, De Corte W. Acceptance of pain is an independent predictor of mental well-being in patients with chronic pain: empirical evidence and reappraisal. Pain 2003;106:65–72, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00291-4.Search in Google Scholar

[131] *Vitiello MV, McCurry SM, Shortreed SM, Baker LD, Rybarczyk BD, Keefe FJ, Von Korff M. Short-term improvement in insomnia symptoms predicts long-term improvements in sleep, pain, and fatigue in older adults with comorbid osteoarthritis and insomnia. Pain 2014;155:1547–54, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.032.Search in Google Scholar

[132] *Vlaeyen JWS, Kole-Snijders AMJ, Boeren RGB, van Eek H. Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain 1995;62:363–72, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(94)00279-N.Search in Google Scholar

[133] *Vlaeyen JW, Kole-Snijders AM, Rotteveel AM, Ruesink R, Heuts PH. The role of fear of movement/(re)injury in pain disability. J Occup Rehabil 1995;5:235–52, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02109988.Search in Google Scholar

[134] *Vowles KE [Doctoral dissertation] Pain-related acceptance and physical impairment in individuals with chronic pain; 2004. Retrieved from ProQuest. (3159358).Search in Google Scholar

[135] *Vowles KE, McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Patient functioning and catastrophizing in chronic pain: the mediating effects of acceptance. Health Psychol 2008;27:S136–43, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.27.2(Suppl.).S136.Search in Google Scholar

[136] *Wicksell RK, Olsson GL, Melin L. The Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ)-further validation including a confirmatory factor analysis and a comparison with the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia. Eur J Pain 2009;13:760–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.09.003.Search in Google Scholar

[137] *Wilson AC, Lewandowski AS, Palermo TM. Fear-avoidance beliefs and parental responses to pain in adolescents with chronic pain. Pain Res Manage 2011;16:178–82.Search in Google Scholar

[138] *Wittink HM, Rogers WH, Lipman AG, McCarberg BH, Ashburn MA, Oderda GM, Carr DB. Older and younger adults in pain management programs in the United States: differences and similarities. Pain Med 2006;7:151–63, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00113.x.Search in Google Scholar

[139] *Witty TE [Doctoral dissertation] Problem solving appraisal and psychological adjustment of chronic low back pain patients; 1997. Retrieved from ProQuest. (9705632).Search in Google Scholar

[140] *Wong WS, McCracken LM, Fielding R. Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the 20-item Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale (ChPASS-20). J Pain Sympt Manage 2012;43:1131–40, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2011.06.021.Search in Google Scholar

[141] *Woods MP [Doctoral dissertation] Evaluating the efficacy of graded in vivo exposure for the treatment of fear in patients with chronic back pain: a randomized controlled clinical trial; 2007. Retrieved from ProQuest. (NR18870).Search in Google Scholar

[142] Kori SH, Miller RP, Todd DD. Kinesiophobia: a new view of chronic pain behavior. Pain Manage 1990:35–43.Search in Google Scholar

[143] McCracken LM, Zayfert C, Gross RT. The pain anxiety symptoms scale: development and validation of a scale to measure fear of pain. Pain 1992;50:67–73, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(92)90113-P.Search in Google Scholar

[144] Zarkowska AW. The relationship between subjective and behavioral aspects of pain in people suffering from lower back pain (thesis). London, UK: University of London; 1981.Search in Google Scholar

[145] Simons LE, Sieberg CB, Carpino E, Logan D, Berde C. The Fear of Pain Questionnaire (FOPQ): assessment of pain-related fear among children and adolescents with chronic pain. J Pain 2011;12:677–86, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2010.12.008.Search in Google Scholar

[146] Crowley D, Kendall NA. Development and initial validation of a questionnaire for measuring fear-avoidance associated with pain: the fear-avoidance of pain scale. J Musculoskeletal Pain 1999;7:3–19, http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J094v07n03_02.Search in Google Scholar

[147] Cane D, Nielson WR, Ivey B, Ivey R, McCarthy M, Mazmanian D. Development and preliminary evaluation of a measure of three pain-related patterns of activity. Pain Res Manage 2007;12:124.Search in Google Scholar

[148] Schermelleh-Engel K. Questionnaire on pain regulation (FSR). Frankfurt, Germany: Swets and Zeitlinger; 1995.Search in Google Scholar

[149] Namir S, Wolcott DL, Fawzy IF, Alumbaugh MJ. Coping with AIDS: psychological and health implications. J Appl Soc Psychol 1987;17:309–28, http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1987.tb00316.x.Search in Google Scholar

[150] Philips H, Jahanshahi M. The components of pain behaviour report. Behav Res Ther 1986;24:117–25, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(86)90082-3.Search in Google Scholar

[151] Rogers WH, Wittink HM, Ashburn MA, Cynn D, Carr DB. Using the “TOPS”, an outcome instrument for multidisciplinary outpatient pain treatment. Pain Med 2000;1:55–67, http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1526-4637.2000.99101.x.Search in Google Scholar

[152] McCracken LM, Samuel VM. The role of avoidance, pacing, and other activity patterns in chronic pain. Pain 2007;130:119–25, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2006.11.016.Search in Google Scholar

[153] McCracken LM, Vowles KE. Psychological flexibility and traditional pain management strategies in relation to patient functioning with chronic pain: an examination of a revised instrument. J Pain 2007;8:700–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.04.008.Search in Google Scholar

[154] Kraaimaat F, Evers A. Pain coping strategies in chronic pain patients: psychometric characteristics of the Pain Coping Inventory (PCI). Int J Behav Med 2003;41:1295–310, http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327558IJBM1004_5.Search in Google Scholar

[155] Chambless D, Caputo G, Bright P, Gallagher R. Assessment of fear of fear in agoraphobics: the body sensations questionnaire and the agoraphobic cognitions questionnaire. J Consult Clin Psychol 1984;52:1090–7, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.52.6.1090.Search in Google Scholar

[156] Heppner PP. The problem solving inventory: manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1988.Search in Google Scholar

[157] Endler NS, Parker JD. Coping inventory for stressful situations (CISS): manual. 2nd ed. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 1999.Search in Google Scholar

[158] Folkman S, Lazarus RS. Ways of coping questionnaire. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1988.Search in Google Scholar

[159] Jensen MP, Karoly P. Self-report scales and procedures for assessing pain in adults. In: Turk DC, Melzack R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment. New York: Guildford Press; 1992. p. 135–51.Search in Google Scholar

[160] Melzack R. The McGill pain questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods. Pain 1975;1:277–99, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(75)90044-5.Search in Google Scholar

[161] Kerns RD, Turk DC, Rudy TE. The West Haven—Yale multidimensional pain inventory (WHYMPI). Pain 1985;23:345–56, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(85)90004-1.Search in Google Scholar

[162] Cleeland CS, Ryan KM. Pain assessment: global use of the brief pain inventory. Ann Acad Med 1994;23:129–38.Search in Google Scholar

[163] Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM, Fisher LD. Comparative reliability and validity of chronic pain intensity measures. Pain 1999;83:157–62, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00101-3.Search in Google Scholar

[164] Tearnan BH, Lewandowski MJ. Behavioral assessment of pain. Reno, NV: Sierra Pain Institute; 1988.Search in Google Scholar

[165] Blanchard EB, Theobald DE, Williamson DA, Silver BV, Brown DA. Temperature biofeedback in the treatment of migraine headaches. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978;35:581–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1978.01770290063006.Search in Google Scholar

[166] Ware JE, Sherbourne CD. The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36). I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care 1992;30:473–83, http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002.Search in Google Scholar

[167] Tursky B, Jamner LD, Friedman R. The pain perception profile: a psychological approach to the assessment of pain report. Behav Ther 1982;13:376–94, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(82)80002-6.Search in Google Scholar

[168] Jensen MP, Karoly P, Braver S. The measurement of clinical pain—intensity: a comparison of six methods. Pain 1986;27:117–26, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(86)90228-9.Search in Google Scholar

[169] Von Korff M, Ormel J, Keefe FJ, Dworkin SF. Grading the severity of chronic pain. Pain 1992;50:133–49, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(92)90154-4.Search in Google Scholar

[170] McCaffery M, Beebe A. Pain: clinical manual for nursing practice. Baltimore, MD: V.V. Mosby Company; 1993.Search in Google Scholar

[171] Meenan RF, Gertman PM, Mason JH, Dunaif R. The arthritis impact measurement scales. Further investigations of a health status measure. Arthritis Rheum 1982;25:1048–53.Search in Google Scholar

Appendix A

Major Characteristics of Included Studies.

References n Sample r F-A measure(s) Pain intensity measure(s) Pain Duration %Married/ Cohabiting Age %Male Country
Asghari and Nicholas [48] 145 Chronic pain .2 PBQ NRS 188.8 71 50.3 41 Australia
Asmundson et al (1999) 72 Chronic headaches .39 PASS HQ 193.1 43.1 15 Canada
Barke et al. (2015) 182 CBP .24 CPAQ NRS 152.4 51 29.67 Germany
Bendayan et al. [49] 315 Chronic pain .41 CPAQ, AAQ NRS 127 69.6 53 25.1 Spain
Boersma and Linton [50] 141 Chronic back/neck pain .21 TSK NRS 47.7 20 Sweden
Callahan [51] 84 TMD facial pain .09 WOC VRS 71 37 7 USA
Cannon [52] 333 Chronic pain .21 PBC MPI 79 43.5 46 Canada
Carleton [53] 31 Chronic pain .42 PASS, FABQ MPQ-SF 46.19 41.9 Canada
Carleton et al. [54] 68 CLBP .29 PASS-20, FABQ VAS 23.93 56 40.2 65 Canada
Carleton et al. [55] 15 Fibromyalgia .01 PASS-20 VAS 147.6 52.5 6 Canada
Carranza [56] 48 Chronic headache .08 PASS, PBQ HI 6 22 6 USA
Cascarilla [57] 117 Chronic pain .03 CPAQ NRS 131.76 62.4 45.7 36.8 USA
Chiu [58] 41 Chronic pain .5 TOPS-FA TOPS-Pain 75.61 43.1 36.6 USA
Cho et al. [59] 166 Chronic pain .5 PASS-20 NRS 36 74.1 48.7 29.5 Korea
Cho et al. [60] 142 Chronic pain .38 PASS-20 SF-36 BP 32 58 44.5 36.6 Korea
Combs & Thorn (2015) 102 CBP .43 TSK NRS 143.58 50.5 39.2 USA
Cook et al. [61] 469 Chronic pain .3 TSK MPQ 74.4 51 46.3 36 USA
Crombez et al. [62], 38 Chronic pain .22 TSK VAS 78.72 78.95 36.84 28.95 Belgium
Crombez et al. [63]
Crombez et al. [63]. 35 CBP .19 FABQ, TSK VAS 80.4 36.1 32 Belgium
Sample 1
Crombez et al. [63]. 38 CBP .38 TSK, FABQ VAS 76.2 40.84 34 Belgium
Sample 2
Crombez et al. [63]. 31 CBP .24 TSK, PASS VAS 121.2 41.61 48 Netherlands
Sample 3
Crowley and Kendall 53 Chronic pain .28 FAPS VAS, MPI 48.8 32 35.7 33 New Zealand
[64]
Cui et al. [65] 63 Chronic pain -.29 CISS-A VAS 46.1 34.92 59.3 34.92 Japan
Dehghani et al. [66] 161 Chronic pain .41 TSK MPI 74 61 44.56 Australia
Dehghani et al. [15] 207 Chronic pain .37 TSK MPI 75 56 45.2 46.9 Australia
Denison et al. [67]. Sample 1 210 Chronic pain .23 TSK NRS 12 74 45 24 Sweden
Denison et al. [67]. Sample 2 218 Chronic pain .23 TSK NRS 12 69 47 35 Sweden
Dyson [68] 206 CBP .06 PASS VRS 42.8 67 USA
Endler et al. [69] 147 Chronic pain .19 CISS-A MPI, MPQ 31.93 Canada
Esteve and Ramirez-Maestre [70]. Sample 1 128 Irritable bowel syndrome .38 AAQ, PASS, CPAQ PII, SF-36 BP 63.88 51 37.91 45.31 Spain
Esteve and Ramirez-Maestre [70]. Sample 2 141 CBP .36 AAQ, PASS, CPAQ PII, SF-36 BP 53.05 69 45.75 45.39 Spain
Esteve and Ramirez-Maestre [70]. Sample 3 137 Chronic pain .46 AAQ, PASS, CPAQ PII, SF-36 BP 107.16 76 53.2 43.8 Spain
Esteve et al. [71] 299 CBP .2 AAQ, FABQ PII 25.21 61.5 44.18 46.2 Spain
Esteve et al. (2013) 468 CBP .24 FABQ, PASS, CPAQ PII 70.11 69.8 46.54 40.6 Spain
Esteve et al. (2015) 111 Chronic pain .19 POAM NRS 176.28 79.3 53 30 Spain
Farin et al. [72] 688 CBP .26 FABQ VAS 74.8 51 42.8 Germany
Fersum et al. [73] 94 CBP .27 FABQ PINRS 132.58 41.87 49.94 Norway
Fish et al. [74]. 428 Chronic pain .38 CPAQ BPI 147.61 69.86 46.99 19.39 Ireland, UK, US, Australia
Foote [75] 103 Migraine .37 PASS-20, CPAQ HS 41.45 11.8 USA
French et al. [76] 200 CBP, Neck pain .26 TSK, FABQ VAS 15.41 40 46 Canada
Geiser [44] 65 Chronic pain .03 APQ BAP 31 41.5 46.2 USA
Gomez-Perez et al. [77] 125 Chronic pain .27 TSK PII 140.64 58.26 19.2 Spain
Goubert et al. [78] 122 CBP .29 TSK MPI 97.98 84.2 43.72 39.3 Netherlands
Goubert et al. [79] 39 CBP .24 TSK, PASS MPI 93.01 79.5 43.49 25.6 Belgium
Grotle et al. [80] 233 CLBP .22 FABQ VAS 42 46 Norway
Harrison et al. (2015) 608 Multiple sclerosis .24 AEQ BPI 153.6 52.4 25.7 England
Jacox [81] 293 Endometriosis .15 DICI VAS 61.65 34.18 0 USA
Jeffrey [82] 96 Rheumatoid arthritis -.11 WOC VAS 12.2 56.3 48.1 12 USA
Kindermans et al. [83] 132 Chronic pain .28 POAM, PARQ VAS 146.82 45.62 34.01 Netherlands
Knaster et al. [84] 121 Chronic pain .04 PASS-20 VAS 48 60 47.9 38 Finland
Koho et al. [85] 51 Chronic pain .41 TSK VAS 69.7 44.6 47.06 Finland
Lamoth et al. [86] 19 CBP .58 TSK VAS 14.4 38 42.1 Netherlands
Larsen et al. [87] 259 Chronic pain .22 PASS MPQ 21 38.2 58 Canada
Larsson et al. [88] 433 Chronic pain .31 TSK NRS 122.4 54.3 74.8 36.5 Sweden
Laufer et al. [89] 63 Chronic pain .36 FABQ VAS 46.3 56 Israel
Lee [90] 168 Chronic pain .34 FABQ NRS 42.81 36.4 China
Lopez-Martinez et al. [91] 149 Chronic pain .27 PASS-20 PII 59.38 55 46.83 25.5 Spain
Luedtke et al. [92] 135 CBP .14 FABQ VAS 96 45 54.07 Germany
Lundberg et al. [93]. Sample 1 73 CBP .29 TSK VAS 57.53 46.4 42.47 Sweden
Lundberg et al. [93]. Sample 2 74 CBP .2 TSK VAS 55.41 46.7 47.3 Sweden
Macleod [94] 117 TMJ pain .2 PASS GCPS 87.8 62.4 39.4 11 USA
Martin et al. [95] 208 Chronic pain .2 PASS-20 NRS 66.72 47.18 40.01 Canada
Martin et al. [96] 21 Chronic pain -.04 PASS-20 NRS 14.2 23.81 Canada
McCracken [97] 235 Chronic pain .19 CPAQ, PASS NRS 119.5 65.2 38.2 Britain
McCracken et al. [98] 122 Chronic pain .08 CPAQ NRS 54 15.2 24.6 UK
McCracken and Eccleston [99] 230 Chronic pain .26 CPAQ, PASS VAS 32.5 56.1 46.4 33.5 Britain
McCracken and Keogh [100] 125 Chronic pain .34 CPAQ, PASS-20 NRS 96 63.2 46.6 35.2 Britain
McCracken and Velleman [22] 239 Chronic pain .41 AAQ, CPAQ SF-36 120 61.8 61.5 41.8 Britain
McCracken and Zhao-O’Brien [23] 144 Chronic pain .15 AAQ-II, CPAQ, PASS-20 NRS 139 58.4 42.4 36.1 Britain
McCracken et al. [101] 105 Chronic pain .29 CPAQ, PASS-20 NRS 96 61.9 46.9 40 UK
McCracken et al. [8] 45 Chronic pain .42 FABQ, PASS, PBC MPQ 27 68.9 46.3 46.7 USA
McCracken et al. [102] 190 Chronic pain .23 CPAQ, PASS MPI 36 56.8 47.1 33.7 Britain
Meyer [103] 139 Chronic pain .24 CPAQ, AAQ-II VAS 83.28 20.9 45.43 38.8 USA
Meyer et al. [104] 111 CBP .43 FABQ NRS 49 32 Switzerland
Michalski et al. [105] 38 Multiple sclerosis .12 FSR NRS 103.2 42 18.4 Germany
Newton-John et al. [106] 101 Chronic pain .32 TSK NRS 33.91 43 56.4 Australia
Nicholas and Asghari [107] 252 Chronic pain .24 TSK, CPAQ MPI 93.7 50.33 37.7 Australia
Nicholas et al. [108] 567 Chronic pain .36 TSK MPI 31.2 55.5 44 48 Australia
O’Connell [109] 91 Chronic pain .09 WOC MPI 61.2 70 45 42.86 USA
Ojala et al. [110] 81 Chronic pain .25 CPAQ VAS 51.6 64 49 37 Finland
Perry and Francis [111] 68 Chronic pain .48 TSK, FABQ BPI 116.04 43.47 26 Australia
Plumb Vilardaga [112] 28 Chronic pain .46 PIPS, BPCI PIR 17.14 58 52.59 25 USA
Ramírez-Maestre and Esteve [113]. 190 Chronic spinal pain .33 CPAQ, PASS PII 70.58 72.2 46.28 100 Spain
Ramírez-Maestre and Esteve [113]. Sample 1 210 Chronic spinal pain .34 CPAQ, PASS PII 63.26 69.5 46.17 0 Spain
Ramírez-Maestre et al. [16] 686 Chronic spinal pain .28 AAQ, FABQ, CPAQ PII 48.7 60.3 45.4 41 Spain
Riecke et al. [114] 111 CBP .08 PASS, TSK NRS 169.92 71.2 51.92 45.5 Germany
Rodero et al. [115] 205 Fibromyalgia .45 CPAQ VAS 145.2 73.6 50 9.3 Spain
Ruiz-Parraga & Lopez-Martinez (2015) 229 CBP .27 CPAQ PII 55.32 66.8 45.53 28.8 Spain
Ruiz-Parraga et al. (2015) 150 Chronic pain .55 AEQ PII 92.4 62 48.27 38 Spain
Samwel et al. [116] 181 Chronic pain .22 TSK, PCI VAS 64.1 76.9 48.7 35.9 Netherlands
Samwel et al. [117] 169 Chronic pain .24 TSK VAS 59.9 79.1 47.1 36.1 Netherlands
Schutze et al. [118] 104 Chronic pain .32 TSK BPI 125.7 54.5 31.7 Australia
Sil et al. [119] 8 Fibromyalgia .65 TSK VAS 15.88 USA
Simons and Kaczynski [120] 275 Chronic pain .24 FOPQ NRS 13 13.73 25.2 USA
Simons et al. [121] 181 Chronic pain .2 FOPQ NRS 19 13.6 27.2 USA
Simons, Smith, Kaczynski, & Basch (2014) 312 Chronic pain .15 FOPQ NRS 14 13.7 19.4 USA
Strahl et al. [122] 154 Rheumatoid arthritis .19 PASS AIMS-2 177.84 55 54.07 13.64 USA
Sullivan et al. [123]. 226 Chronic pain .33 TSK MPQ 39.78 40.6 Canada
Suttenfield [124] 95 Chronic pain .46 PSITQ MPQ 133 48 30.5 USA
Thomas [125] 148 Chronic pain .23 CPAQ, AAQ MPI 201.84 68.9 47 15 USA
Tkachuk and Harris [126] 276 Chronic pain .27 TSK, CPAQ MPI 102 47.76 35 Canada
Trompetter et al. [127] 428 Chronic pain .25 PIPS NRS 69.3 43.7 27.8 Netherlands
Trost et al. [128] 60 CBP .19 TSK MPQ 98.4 31.9 38.33 USA
Turk et al. [129] 233 Fibromyalgia .2 TSK MPI 123.6 53.6 43.79 0 USA
Viane et al. [130]. Study 1 120 Chronic pain .12 CPAQ MPI 87.56 56.7 41.46 18.33 Belgium
Viane et al. [130]. Study 2 66 Chronic pain .45 CPAQ, ICQ MPI 125.98 81.1 46.77 24.24 Belgium
Vitiello et al. [131] 354 Osteoarthritis .22 TSK GCPS 73.1 21.75 USA
Vlaeyen et al. [132]. 33 CBP 0 TSK VAS 91.2 37.4 48.48 Netherlands
Vlaeyen et al. [133]. Study 1 103 Chronic pain .25 TSK VAS 122.07 40.7 43.69 Netherlands
Vlaeyen et al. [133]. Study 2 129 CBP .21 TSK MPQ 118.8 40.1 38.76 Netherlands
Vowles [134] 74 Chronic pain, worker’s comp .43 CPAQ, PASS MPQ 20.9 67.6 38.5 70.3 USA
Vowles et al. [135] 334 Chronic pain CPAQ, PASS NRS 96 61.4 46.2 37.8 UK
Wicksell et al. [136] 611 Whiplash associated disorder .34 CPAQ, TSK NRS 120 79.8 49 25.2 Sweden
Wilson et al. [137] 42 Chronic pain .22 FABQ NRS 14.9 26.2 USA
Wittink et al. [138] 6147 Chronic pain -.03 TOPS TOPS, SF-36 57.33 34.39 USA
Witty [139] 78 CBP .05 PSI MPQ 65 48.39 93.6 USA
Wong et al. [140] 242 Chronic pain .54 PASS-20, TSK CPGQ 47.52 66.9 45.67 41.9 Hong Kong
Woods [141] 44 CBP .42 TSK, FABQ, PASS MPQ 46.45 34.1 Canada
  1. Note. Pain duration is reported in months. Age is reported in years.

    Abbreviations: AAQ Acceptance and Action Questionnaire; ACPT, Acceptance and Control in Psychological Treatment; AIMS-2, Arthritis Impact Scale-2nd edition; BAP, Brief Assessment of Pain; BCPI, Brief Pain Coping Inventory; BPI, Brief Pain Inventory; CBP, Chronic back pain; CISS, Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations; CPAQ Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire; DICI, Dealing with Illness Coping Inventory; FABQ Fear-Avoidance Beliefs; FAPS, Fear Avoidance of Pain Scale; FOPQ Fear of Pain Questionnaire; FSR, Questionnaire on Pain Regulation; GCPS, Graded Chronic Pain Scale; HI, Headache Index; HQ, Headache Questionnaire; MPI, Multidimensional Pain Inventory; MPQ McGill Pain Questionnaire; NRS, Numerical Rating Scale; PARQ Pain and Activity Relations Questionnaire; PASS, Pain Anxiety Symptoms Scale; PBC, Pain Behavior Questionnaire; PBQ Pain Behavior Questionnaire; PCI, Pain Coping Inventory; PII, Pain Intensity Index; PINRS, Pain Intensity Numeric Rating Scale; PIR, Pain Intensity Rating; POAM-P, Patterns of Activity Mapping–Pain; PSI, Problem-Solving Inventory; PSITQ; Pain Situations Questionnaire; SF-36, Short Form Health Survey-36; TOPS, Treatment Outcomes of Pain Survey; TSK, Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia; VAS, Visual Analogue Scale; VRS, Verbal Rating Scale; WOC, Ways of Coping.

Received: 2016-03-24
Revised: 2016-06-24
Accepted: 2016-06-27
Published Online: 2016-10-01
Published in Print: 2016-10-01

© 2016 Scandinavian Association for the Study of Pain

Downloaded on 16.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.06.011/html
Scroll to top button