Associations of symptoms and subtypes of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with visuospatial planning ability in youth

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.020Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Inattention was associated with spatial planning impairment.

  • The magnitudes of such an association increased with increased task difficulties.

  • After controlling for age, sex, comorbidity and IQ, these associations remained.

  • Combined and prominently inattentive subtypes had more spatial planning impairment.

Abstract

Little is known about which ADHD core symptom or subtype is most associated with visuospatial planning deficit. This issue was investigated in a sample of 408 youths with current DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD, and 332 youths without lifetime ADHD, aged 8–17 years (mean age 12.02 ± 2.24). All the participants and their mothers were interviewed using the Chinese Kiddie Epidemiologic version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia to obtain information about ADHD symptoms and diagnosis and other psychiatric disorders. In addition to clinical assessments, the participants were assessed with the WISC-III and the Stocking of Cambridge task of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery. Multi-level regression models were used for data analysis. The results showed that univariate analyses revealed that inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity were significantly associated with visuospatial planning, and the magnitude of such association was amplified with increased task difficulties. Only inattention independently predicted visuospatial planning in a model that included all three ADHD symptoms. After further controlling for comorbidity, age of assessment, treatment with methylphenidate, and Full-scale IQ, inattention was still independently associated with visuospatial planning indexed by mean moves needed to solve problems. In subtype comparison, participants with combined subtype and those with prominently inattentive subtype, rather than prominently hyperactivity/impulsivity subtype, had poorer visuospatial planning performance. Our findings indicate that inattention is independently associated with impaired visuospatial planning, and dimensional approach retains the important distinction among ADHD symptoms than subtype approach in understanding the neuropsychological functioning of ADHD.

Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common childhood psychiatric disorders with long-term executive function deficits and functional impairment (Bernardi et al., 2012, Biederman et al., 2007, Faraone et al., 2003, Stavro et al., 2007, Willcutt et al., 2005). Executive functions often include working memory, planning, organization, inhibitory control, sustained attention, and set shifting. Deficits in executive functions negatively impact functional outcome in children (Biederman et al., 2004) and adults (Biederman et al., 2006, Stavro et al., 2007) with ADHD, including declined academic performance, occupational attainments, and adaptive social/leisure functioning. Visuospatial planning deficit is one of the most evident and consistent executive function impairments in ADHD (Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996, Willcutt et al., 2005). Rinsky and Hinshaw (2011) reported that visuospatial planning ability in childhood predicted latter social function in adolescence under tight statistical control. A recent study provided evidence to support that visuospatial planning mediate the effect of ADHD on social problems (Tseng & Gau, 2013). Recent study has provided more collateral evidence that both symptom severity of ADHD and visuospatial planning dysfunction were significantly correlated with decreased integrity of the frontostriatal tracts (Shang, Wu, Gau, & Tseng, 2013).

Although children and adults with ADHD tend to have poorer visuospatial planning and associated functional impairments than healthy controls (Culbertson and Zillmer, 1998, Gau et al., 2009, Gau and Shang, 2010, McLean et al., 2004, Nigg et al., 2002, Nigg et al., 2004, Rhodes et al., 2006, Solanto et al., 2007), impairment of visuospatial planning is not universal among all patients with ADHD as summarized in a meta-analysis (Willcutt et al., 2005). In fact, less than half of children with ADHD are impaired significantly if only a single executive function task is administered (Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005). The clinical predictors for visuospatial planning deficit in children and adolescents with ADHD have not been fully understood.

According to the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria, three ADHD subtypes are categorized: predominantly inattentive subtype (ADHD-I), predominantly hyperactive/impulsive subtype (ADHD-HI) and combined subtype (ADHD-C) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) based on three ADHD core symptoms: inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity. Studies on visuospatial planning among the three ADHD subtypes revealed inconsistent results that the ADHD-C might have more (Klorman et al., 1999, Kopecky et al., 2005) or similar (Nigg et al., 2002, Pasini et al., 2007) impairment as compared with the ADHD-I, followed by the ADHD-HI, the least (Chhabildas et al., 2001, Schmitz et al., 2002). Using dimensional approach, inattention, rather than hyperactivity/impulsivity, was consistently associated with executive dysfunction (Martel et al., 2007, Martinussen and Tannock, 2006, Willcutt et al., 2012), and more related to functional impairment (Stavro et al., 2007). However, few studies controlling hyperactivity and impulsivity when examining the relations between inattention and executive deficits, or vice versa (Brocki et al., 2010, Chhabildas et al., 2001); and no such study focusing on visuospatial planning has been reported.

The inconsistent results in previous studies could be explained by small sample sizes (Geurts et al., 2005, Houghton et al., 1999), exclusion of ADHD-HI in subtype approach (Geurts et al., 2005, Houghton et al., 1999, Kopecky et al., 2005, Pasini et al., 2007), lack of careful control of possible confounding factors, such as age (Culbertson & Zillmer, 1998), IQ (Scheres et al., 2004, Solanto et al., 2007), gender (Seidman et al., 1997), psychiatric comorbidity (Klorman et al., 1999), and no identification of specific ADHD symptoms (using categorical approach with or without ADHD diagnosis) (Gau et al., 2009).

To the best of our knowledge, none of the previous report has comprehensively examined clinical predictors of visuospatial planning deficit. Therefore, using a large ethnic-Chinese sample, we intend to identify specific ADHD symptoms associated with visuospatial planning deficit by separating ADHD core symptoms into three dimensions, i.e., inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, and by comparing the three ADHD subtypes. We also examined the effect of task difficulties, and applied stringent control of confounding factors in the statistical model. We hypothesized that inattention was mostly associated with poor visuospatial planning after controlling for all possible confounders and the magnitude of association would increase with higher task difficulties, and that youths with ADHD-C and ADHD-I who had prominent inattentive symptoms performed worse in visuospatial planning than youths without ADHD.

Section snippets

Participants

There were 790 participants in total (559 males, 70.8%) with a mean age of 12.02 (standard deviation, SD = 2.24). Among them, 422 children (aged 8–17) had parent-reported overt ADHD symptoms at the mean age of 4.83 (SD = 2.02), who were clinically diagnosed with ADHD according to the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria at the mean age of 8.51 (SD = 3.02), and were further assessed by using the Chinese version of the Kiddie epidemiologic version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia

Sample characteristics

Among the 790 participants, 401 (50.7%) were patients with ADHD who had been diagnosed by child psychiatrists at earlier childhood before the assessments, 213 (27.0%) siblings of patients with ADHD, and 176 (22.3%) school children without lifetime ADHD. There were 559 boys (70.8%) and 231 girls (29.2%), with mean age of 12.02 (SD = 2.24, range 8–17). The distributions of current ADHD subtype, medication history, parental characteristics are shown in Table 1. The weighted kappa value was 0.90 for

Discussion

This is the first study to comprehensively examine clinical predictors, including ADHD symptoms and subtypes, for visuospatial planning deficit in children and adolescents with ADHD. We found that inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity were significantly associated with visuospatial planning, and the magnitude of such association was amplified with increased task difficulties. Our novel finding is that inattention, rather than hyperactivity and impulsivity, independently predicts

Conclusions

As the first study to identify the specific ADHD core symptoms and subtypes to be associated with visuospatial planning ability using hierarchical multiple regression models controlling confounding factors, we found inattention symptoms and both ADHD-C and ADHD-I were significantly associated with visuospatial planning. Such associations were stronger with increased task difficulties. The clinical implication is that despite evidence of executive dysfunctions in ADHD, clinical resources are

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from the National Health Research Institute (NHRI-EX94-9407PC, NHRI-EX95-9407PC, NHRI-EX96-9407PC, NHRI-EX97-9407PC), and the National Science Council (NSC96-2628-B-002-069-MY3), Taiwan. The authors would like to thank Ming-Fang Chen for her assistance in data analysis and all the participants and their parents and teachers for their contribution.

References (61)

  • A. Scheres et al.

    Executive functioning in boys with ADHD: Primarily an inhibition deficit?

    Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • L.J. Seidman et al.

    A pilot study of neuropsychological function in girls with ADHD

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • E.G. Willcutt et al.

    Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (1994)
  • R. Barkley

    Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1997)
  • S. Bernardi et al.

    The lifetime impact of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC)

    Psychological Medicine

    (2012)
  • J. Biederman et al.

    Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2004)
  • J. Biederman et al.

    Impact of psychometrically defined deficits of executive functioning in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • J. Biederman et al.

    Stability of executive function deficits into young adult years: A prospective longitudinal follow-up study of grown up males with ADHD

    Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica

    (2007)
  • A.R. Borkowska et al.

    Visual-spatial functions and organisation of grapho-motor actions in ADHD children

    Psychiatria Polska

    (2011)
  • K.C. Brocki et al.

    Interrelations between executive function and symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention in preschoolers: A two year longitudinal study

    Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology

    (2010)
  • N. Chhabildas et al.

    A comparison of the neuropsychological profiles of the DSM-IV subtypes of ADHD

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2001)
  • A. Doyle et al.

    Are endophenotypes based on measures of executive functions useful for molecular genetic studies of ADHD

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines

    (2005)
  • S.V. Faraone et al.

    The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: Is it an American condition?

    World Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    Sleep problems and disorders among adolescents with persistent and sub-threshold attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders

    Sleep

    (2009)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    Executive function in adolescence among children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Taiwan

    Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics

    (2009)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    A 3-year panel study of mental disorders among adolescents in Taiwan

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    Psychiatric and psychosocial predictors of substance use disorders among adolescents: Longitudinal study

    British Journal of Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    Rapid visual information processing as a cognitive endophenotype of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

    Psychological Medicine

    (2013)
  • S.S. Gau et al.

    Psychopathology and symptom remission at adolescence among children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder

    Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • Cited by (14)

    • Neural substrates underpinning intra-individual variability in children with ADHD: A voxel-based morphometry study

      2022, Journal of the Formosan Medical Association
      Citation Excerpt :

      The Chinese K-SADS-E is a reliable and valid instrument to assess DSM-IV child and adolescent psychiatric disorders.29 It has been widely used in making the DSM-IV psychiatric diagnoses of children and adolescents in epidemiological23,29 and clinical30–32 studies in Taiwan. We used the Chinese K-SADS-E to confirm that the ADHD group still had a current diagnosis of ADHD and the TD group did not have a lifetime diagnosis of ADHD.

    • Neurological soft signs, but not theory of mind and emotion recognition deficit distinguished children with ADHD from healthy control

      2017, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      In a previous study Chiang and colleagues investigated the correlation between ADHD core symptoms and planning. The authors demonstrated that inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity were significantly associated with planning deficit and only inattention independently predicted planning in a model that included all three ADHD symptoms and subtypes (Chiang et al., 2013). As starting hypothesis, in the present study ADHD group showed a greater number of NSS compared to control group.

    • Neurocognitive characteristics of youth with noncomorbid and comorbid forms of conduct disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

      2017, Comprehensive Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Spatial planning is an executive function that is thought to rely heavily on frontal lobe functioning [40,41]. Deficits in spatial planning have been associated with greater severity of ADHD symptoms [22]. Poorer spatial planning abilities in youth with ADHD have been associated with more negative peer relationships [23] and have been found to mediate the relationship between ADHD and social problems [24].

    • Impaired sustained attention, focused attention, and vigilance in youths with autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder

      2014, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Their parents also received psychiatric interview by using the Chinese Kiddie epidemiologic version of the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS-E) interview for the diagnosis of ADHD and other psychiatric disorders. The reliability and validity of the Chinese K-SADS-E is established (Gau, Chong, Chen, & Cheng, 2005), and has been extensively used in assessing childhood mental disorders (Chiang, Huang, Gau, & Shang, 2013; Chien et al., 2012; Gau & Chiang, 2013; Gau & Huang, 2013; Hwang Gu, Gau, Tzang, & Hsu, 2013; Lin et al., 2013; Shang, Gau, Liu, & Hwu, 2011; Shang, Wu, Gau, & Tseng, 2013; Wu, Gau, Lo, & Tseng, 2014). The 255 TD youths were recruited at schools in the same districts of the ASD groups through the teachers and principals rather than through advertisement.

    • Impact of executive functions on school and peer functions in youths with ADHD

      2014, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, deficits in spatial working memory and spatial planning have been demonstrated with the largest effect sizes between the ADHD and control groups in our pilot study (Gau et al., 2009) and endophenotype study (Gau & Shang, 2010a). Second, deficits in these two EFs are frequently noted in ADHD and associated with severity of ADHD symptoms (Chiang, Huang, Gau, & Shang, 2013; Gau & Chiang, 2013). Third, based on the analysis using adolescents with persistent, subsyndromal and remitted attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, these two EFs are independent of intelligence and other neuropsychological function with low executive demand (e.g., response inhibition) (H.Y. Lin et al., 2013; Y.J. Lin, Chen, & Gau, 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text