Elsevier

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Volume 33, Issue 1, January–February 2012, Pages 274-282
Research in Developmental Disabilities

Principal component analysis study of visual and verbal metaphoric comprehension in children with autism and learning disabilities

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.09.010Get rights and content

Abstract

This research extends previous studies regarding the metaphoric competence of autistic and learning disable children on different measures of visual and verbal non-literal language comprehension, as well as cognitive abilities that include semantic knowledge, executive functions, similarities, and reading fluency. Thirty seven children with autism (ASD), 20 children with learning disabilities (LD), and 21 typically developed (TD) children participated in the study. Principal components analysis was used to examine the interrelationship among the various tests in each group. Results showed different patterns in the data according to group. In particular, the results revealed that there is no dichotomy between visual and verbal metaphors in TD children but rather metaphor are classified according to their familiarity level. In the LD group visual metaphors were classified independently of the verbal metaphors. Verbal metaphoric understanding in the ASD group resembled the LD group. In addition, our results revealed the relative weakness of the ASD and LD children in suppressing irrelevant information.

Highlights

► Metaphors are classified according to their familiarity level in TD children. ► Metaphors are classified according to their type: visual or verbal in LD children. ► Most of the variance in the TD group is explained by the semantic knowledge. ► Results revealed the weakness of ASD and LD in suppressing meaningless information.

Introduction

Metaphors play a natural and often a necessary role in the mundane language of everyday interaction. Developing metaphor competence enables children to use metaphor for reaching shared understandings and for learning. It has been suggested that the processes underlying metaphor are basic to human development from infancy and that “metaphor provides one of the basic ways of learning about the world. Its mapping of different, previously unrelated domains onto each other is seen to extend children's knowledge to that which is unfamiliar, thus making it a tool as well as a skill” (Marschark & Nall, 1985, p. 54). Metaphor understanding gradually develops throughout childhood into adulthood simultaneously with other cognitive capacities as well with the development of world knowledge (Vosniadou & Ortony, 1983). It involves awareness of similarities and relations between different domains and the integration of analogous elements into novel concept. Keil (1986) argued that to understand metaphors one must have enough knowledge to discover a similarity between the two concepts involved (the topic and the base term of the metaphoric expression). It is therefore not surprising that these requirements impose considerably figurative language impairments on special populations, such as autism and learning disabilities, which results in a tendency to interpret figurative utterances literally (e.g., Friemoth Lee and Kamhi, 1990, Roundblad and Annaz, 2010). Norbury (2005) found that ASD children with language deficits were impaired on metaphor comprehension task and that they were indistinguishable from non-autistic children with language impairment, suggesting that semantic knowledge is a key determinant of metaphor comprehension. In the current study we aim to deepen our understanding of the relationships between metaphoric competence and various cognitive abilities in children with autism and learning disabilities and to compare them to those of typically developed (TD) children.

Accumulating evidence suggests that children with autism (ASD) exhibit figurative language comprehension deficits and especially metaphoric impairments (Happé, 1993, Happé, 1995, MacKay and Shaw, 2004, Roundblad and Annaz, 2010). Some researchers have reported similar non-literal language comprehension deficits in LD children (e.g., Friemoth Lee and Kamhi, 1990, Nippold and Fey, 1983). However, studies that directly compared figurative language comprehension in both ASD and LD children are remarkably scarce. A recent study that compared non-literal language comprehension as well as performances in fluency tests and homophone meaning generation test in both individuals with autism (aged 12–15) and learning disabilities (aged 12–13) demonstrated difficulty in interpreting non-literal expressions in multiple choice tests as compared with TD children (aged 12–13) (Mashal & Kasirer, 2011). Significant differences were found in the understanding of idioms (e.g., “he got cold feet”), conventional metaphors (e.g., “defense line”), meaningless word pairs (e.g., “sport lemon”). ASD and LD children provided more literal interpretations to idioms and in addition, interpreted meaningless two word expressions more often than TD children. No differences were observed between the ASD and the LD group indicating a similar non-literal language profile for both groups. This finding pinpoint the difficulty of both groups to access and select relevant and affective conceptual information about topic and base term domains, and to construct links that enable resolution of the incongruity between the topic and the base term. For example, understanding the metaphoric expression “Family is a cradle”, in which “family” represents the topic and “cradle” the base term, requires the generation of a broad range of associations to each concept and then, the simultaneously selection of the appropriate shared properties, such as safety and comfort and the suppressions of the irrelevant ones (e.g., swinging) (an example adopted from Lundgren, Brownel, Soma, & Cayer-Meade, 2006).

The source of this metaphorical difficulty among ASD and LD children is still unknown. It could be that the metaphoricity per-se is intact but the use verbal stimuli exerts additional cognitive demand on children with autism and especially, children with learning disability, who suffer from reading impairments. The occurrence of metaphors is not restricted to language and the verbal domain but rather metaphors are pervasive in pictures, commercials, movie scenes, and art. Visual metaphors offer a non-verbal means to convey metaphorical messages that may be beneficial for some individuals because it engages a process of adapting information from an external visual structure, that bypass the verbal mediation, to one's internal conceptual structures. Very little is known about the similarities or differences between visual and verbal constructs in the assessment of LD and ASD children metaphoric understanding. Friemoth Lee and Kamhi (1990) studied visual and verbal metaphoric understanding of children with LD, children with LD with a history of spoken language impairments, and TD children ranging in age from 9 to 11 years. Visual metaphoric understanding was tested by the metaphoric triad test (MTT) that was developed by Kogan, Connor, Gross, and Fava (1980). The MTT consists of 29 triads of pictures, each offering three paring possibilities, in which one is metaphoric (e.g., angry man, thunderstorm, man in the rain, in which the pictures of the angry man and the thunderstorm are metaphorically associated). Identification and correct metaphoric explanation contribute to higher scores. The performance of both LD groups on the verbal tasks was poorer than the performance of TD children. Despite the lower scores of the LD children, visual metaphor understanding did not differ between the LD and the TD group. These results suggest that LD children benefit more from pictorial metaphors than verbal metaphors. In the current study we aimed to examine the relationship between visual and verbal metaphors understanding in both ASD and LD children and in particular, to assess whether visual metaphoric comprehension is a distinct capacity than verbal metaphors.

Autism is a distinct disorder than learning disability, with the former identified as a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by debilitating socioemotional impairments, and the last is identified as perceptual disabilities, brain injury, minimal brain dysfunction, and dyslexia. Although the etiology of these disorders differ, some researchers have shown that in addition to the shared figurative language comprehension deficits both groups demonstrate some inferior executive function performances (Cutting et al., 2009, Mashal and Kasirer, 2011, Minshew et al., 1992, Russell, 1997, Whitney et al., 2009). Executive function consists of higher order skills such as inhibition, working memory, planning, mental flexibility, organizing, and self-monitoring, presumably subserved by the frontal lobes, as evidenced by lesion studies showing that patients with prefrontal lesions have deficits in these areas of functioning (Shallice, 1982). On tests of word fluency where one must produce as many words as possible in one minute, ASD have been shown to be impaired in relation to non-autistic, age, and ability matched controls (Minshew et al., 1992). Executive dysfunction seems to underlie many of the key symptoms of autism, both in the social and in the non-social domains. For example, Turner (1997) reported an association between poor performance on ideational and design fluency tasks and high levels of repetitive behavior in daily life. Mashal and Kasirer (2011) found an association between both phonemic fluency test and homophone meaning generation test and novel metaphors comprehension in ASD children. However, not all studies found poorer performances of ASD compared to TD children in fluency tests (e.g., Boucher, 1988): ASD children were unimpaired in relation to both learning-disabled and age-matched children when generating words belonging to a specific category (e.g., colors, animals). As for LD children, some evidence suggests that executive functions account for a substantial amount of variance in reading comprehension. A recent study that investigated children with adequate word reading accuracy but who develop specific reading comprehension deficits found an association between inferior executive functions and reading comprehension (Cutting et al., 2009). Thus, both ASD and LD exhibit similar profile in figurative language understanding and there is evidence that both are impaired in executive functions. Linking underlying cognitive processes and in particular, executive dysfunctions associated with ASD and LD to the insufficient body of literature on visual and verbal metaphor comprehension is essential for ultimately understanding underlying processes of metaphor comprehension in these special populations.

The current research extends previous works regarding metaphor comprehension and executive dysfunctions in ASD and LD to visual metaphors and to a larger sample of ASD children (Mashal and Kasirer, 2011, Mashal and Kasirer, submitted for publication). In order to gain further insights about the relationship between metaphoric competence, both visual and verbal, and executive functions in ASD and LD as compared with TD children we used the principal component analysis (PCA) method. PCA is a way of identifying patterns (i.e., clusters) in data, and expressing the data in a way that highlights their similarities and differences. This is done by extracting lines (factors) that characterize the data and then transforming the data so that it is expressed in terms of these lines. The goal of the current study was to obtain significant information about the organization of ASD and LD children's metaphor competence compared to TD children. In particular we aimed to (1) examine whether visual metaphors understanding represents a distinct capacity (factor) than verbal metaphors in the TD group and in comparison with the ASD and the LD group; (2) gain deeper insights into the relationships between both visual and verbal metaphor competence and other cognitive abilities that include semantic knowledge, executive functions, similarities, and reading ability that may significantly account for the pattern of variability in these metaphoric capacities.

Section snippets

Participants

The participants of the present study included 78 children that belong to three groups: ASD, LD, and TD children.

The ASD group included 37 children with autism (32 boys and 5 girls; mean age 13.02 years, range 12–15 years). Children were recruited from classes of children with autism (ASD). These classes are integrated within regular junior high schools and located in the center of Israel. Children were selected for the study if (1) they had been diagnosed with autism by a psychiatrist (in line

Group comparisons

Mean correct responses and SDs are summarized in Table 1. Given multiple testing, the threshold of significance was set to p < .01 in the following analyses.

Discussion

The PCA is a useful technique for finding patterns in data of high dimensions, and in expressing the data in a way that highlight their similarities and differences. The first component accounts for most of the variability in the data, and each succeeding component explains the highest variance possible under the constraint that it is orthogonal to (i.e., uncorrelated with) the preceding components. The PCA was applied for each group separately enabling us to gain insights into the different

References (27)

  • F.G.E. Happé

    Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory

    Cognition

    (1993)
  • F. Keil

    Conceptual domains and the acquisition of metaphor

    Cognitive Development

    (1986)
  • J. Boucher

    Word fluency in high-functioning autistic children

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1988)
  • L. Cutting et al.

    Effects of fluency, oral language, and executive function on reading comprehension performance

    Dyslexia

    (2009)
  • C. Forceville

    Metaphor in pictures and multimodal representations

  • R.F. Friemoth Lee et al.

    Metaphoric competence in children with learning disabilities

    Journal of Learning Disabilities

    (1990)
  • I. Glantz

    Hemed

    (2008)
  • F.G.E. Happé

    Understanding minds and metaphors: Insight from the study of figurative language in autism

    Metaphor and Symbolic Activity

    (1995)
  • P.P.M Hurks et al.

    Semantic category fluency versus initial letter fluency over 60 s as a measure of automatic and controlled processing in healthy school-aged children

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2006)
  • G. Kavé et al.

    Switching and clustering in verbal fluency tasks throughout childhood

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • G. Kavé et al.

    Searching for the right word: Performance on four word-retrieval tasks across childhood

    Child Neuropsychology

    (2010)
  • N.K. Kogan et al.

    Understanding visual metaphor: Developmental and individual differences

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1980)
  • K. Lundgren et al.

    A metaphor comprehension intervention for patients with right hemisphere brain damage: A pilot study

    Brain and Language

    (2006)
  • Cited by (37)

    • ACA game for individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder

      2021, Entertainment Computing
      Citation Excerpt :

      Usually behavior patterns are manifested through interest on restricted and/or repetitive stereotyped activities. It is characterized by socialization deficiencies and may present figurative language comprehension deficits [2]. They may also present attention problems and a strange response to the environment and attention stimuli such as sensitivity to noise and touch.

    • Metaphor comprehension in children with and without autism spectrum disorder

      2020, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      In addition, when comparing the groups, children with TD usually obtain better performance than children with ASD on the comprehension of conventional metaphors (Mashal & Kasirer, 2011; Minshew, Goldstein, & Siegal, 1995). However, in the comprehension of novel metaphors, whereas some studies also obtained better performance in children with TD (Chahboun et al., 2016; Gold et al., 2010; Olofson et al., 2014; Van Herwegen & Rundblad, 2018), other studies found that the performance on novel metaphors was similar in both groups (Kasirer & Mashal, 2016; Mashal & Kasirer, 2011, 2012; Melogno, D’Ardia, Pinto, & Levi, 2012; Zheng et al., 2015). The main objective of the present study was to evaluate the metaphor comprehension (both overall and depending on the degree of conventionality/novelty) in a sample of children with and without ASD.

    • Enhancing the comprehension of visual metaphors in individuals with intellectual disability with or without down syndrome

      2018, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      They also exhibit difficulties in executive function skills, particularly working memory, planning (Costanzo et al., 2013; Lanfranchi, Baddeley, Gathercole, & Vianello, 2012) and analogical problem solving (Natsopoulos, Christou, Kiutselini, Raftopoulos, & Karefillidou, 2002). Since metaphor comprehension involves language, executive functions and analogical thinking capacity (Glucksberg, 2008; Gentner & Bowdle, 2008; Holyoak & Thagard, 1989; Mashal & Kasirer, 2011, 2012; Silvia & Beaty, 2012), the cognitive profiles of both etiologies are intriguing, especially with regard to the question of the individuals’ ability to go beyond the concrete level of reasoning and comprehend visual metaphors. The complex nature of figurative language makes it difficult for atypical groups, such as those with ASD (Mackay & Shaw, 2004; Mashal & Kasirer, 2011; Vulchanova, Saldaña, Chahboun, & Vulchanov, 2015), dyslexia (Kasirer & Mashal, 2017) or learning disabilities (Mashal & Kasirer, 2012) to understand.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text