Elsevier

Research in Developmental Disabilities

Volume 32, Issue 6, November–December 2011, Pages 2458-2466
Research in Developmental Disabilities

Quantifying peer interactions for research and clinical use: The Manchester Inventory for Playground Observation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.07.014Get rights and content

Abstract

Direct observation of peer relating is potentially a sensitive and ecologically valid measure of child social functioning, but there has been a lack of standardised methods. The Manchester Inventory for Playground Observation (MIPO) was developed as a practical yet rigorous assessment of this kind for 5–11 year olds. We report on the initial reliability and validity of the MIPO and its ability to distinguish social impairments within different psychopathologies.

We observed 144 clinically referred children aged 5;00–11;11 (mean 8.8) years with Externalising (n = 44), Internalising (n = 19), Autism Spectrum Disorders (n = 39) or Specific Language Impairment (n = 42), and 44 class-controls, in naturalistic playground interaction. Observers, blind to clinical diagnosis, completed the MIPO and the teacher checklist from the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS).

MIPO items showed high internal consistency (alpha = .924; all ‘alpha if item deleted’ values > .91), inter-observer reliability (mean κw = .77) and test–retest stability (over 2 weeks; mean κw = .58). MIPO totals showed convergence with SSRS (n = 68, rs = .78, p < .01) and excellent discrimination between case and control (sensitivity = 0.75 and specificity = 0.88, AUC = .897). Externalising, Autistic Spectrum and Language Impaired groups showed distinct profiles of MIPO impairment consistent with theory:Internalising disorders less so. 65.3% of clinical cases were classified accurately for primary diagnosis.

The MIPO shows reliability and validity as a measure of children's social functioning relevant in developmental research and as a clinical tool to aid differential diagnosis and intervention planning.

Highlights

► There is a need for standard observational assessments of child social functioning. ► MIPO is designed to capture social behaviours relevant to a variety of disorders. ► It shows inter-observer reliability and test–retest stability. ► It discriminates different patterns of social difficulty between common disorders. ► It will have application in research and within clinical assessment.

Introduction

Most forms of developmental disorder or child psychopathology are associated with some form of difficulty in social functioning (Guralnick et al., 2006, Hartup, 2005). The pattern of these difficulties will vary with disorder-type, but are arguably often core to pathogenesis rather than just a functional outcome. Such recognition is hardly new (Sullivan, 1953) but contemporary theory has made the more concrete conceptualisation of ‘social endophenotypes’, intermediate in the pathway to illness expression and a potentially useful marker for illness or proxy target for intervention (Gottesman and Gould, 2003, Skuse et al., 2009). Endophenotypes were originally defined as having an intrinsic or heritable origin (Gottesman & Gould, 2003) but in the context of this paper we propose a broader conceptualisation, which encompasses the variety of social impairment phenotypes seen across childhood development and psychopathology, themselves reflecting different aetiological mechanisms and theories of socialisation. Recent commentary (Banaschewski, 2010) has discussed the implications of this complex variety of social behaviour problems across disorders for multi-level approaches to psychopathology. As examples, we outline below how some different theoretical models of socialisation might predict observed peer social behaviour in children with different disorders and developmental disabilities.

Behaviour genetic studies show that the distributed prosocial and reciprocal social behaviours in the population show significant heritability as well as environmental influence (Plomin, DeFries, & McClearn, 1997). In the highly heritable condition of autism, social impairments are a core characteristic of the disorder (Agam et al., 2003, Baron-Cohen, 2004, Constantino et al., 2004, Holmes and Willoughby, 2005, Hoekstra et al., 2008), with a wide range of problems in reciprocity. Observed peer behaviours in autism will in theory include aloofness and lack of engagement with others, restricted patterns of play, atypical behaviours and self-stimulation. Social impairments are also seen as functional or co-occurring difficulties within a wide range of complex neuro-development disabilities such as Tourette syndrome, ADHD, specific language impairment (SLI) and developmental coordination disorder. In this wider group the social impairment may be intrinsic or a secondary consequence of primary pathology (for instance in SLI, children's interpersonal interactive functioning may be compromised by their difficulties in using higher level language to resolve conflict and assert their interests, Horowitz, Jansson, Ljungberg, & Hedenbro, 2005).

Attachment theory emphasises the motivational and goal orientated aspects of prosocial behaviour and plots how certain patterns of early interaction with caregivers impact the child's social development and response to future social situations (Green, 2009). Primary insecure or disorganised early attachments are associated with cognitive and behavioural social impairments associated with psychopathology (Green and Goldwyn, 2002, Futh et al., 2008) as well as peer rejection (Futh et al., 2008). Here, theory would imply a set of observed peer behaviours around the presence or absence of general pro-social behaviours, and specific difficulties in care-seeking and care-giving, friendship and intimacy. Children with disorganised insecure attachments often develop conduct disorders and their peer interactions may be rather similar to others in that group. In contrast the more severe patterns of Attachment Disorder are characterised by pervasive social impairments and disinhibition—with core difficulties in social awareness, social appropriateness and reciprocal social functioning that can present at the extreme like autism spectrum disorder (O’Connor & Rutter, 2000). Observed peer social behaviour here will be rather different to conduct problems; with interpersonal awkwardness (for instance indiscriminate friendliness) rather than instrumental aggression.

Social learning theory has been key in plotting the pathways into antisocial behaviour via patterns of escalating coercive and hostile interactive behaviours between parents and children. Children who develop conduct disorders commonly have a bias towards reactivity and hostile attributions to others’ actions, which leads them into conflicted and aggressive social behaviour. Their tendency to lack of inhibitory control and their lack of alternative non-aggressive strategies compound the difficulties. Observed peer interactions would in theory reflect these difficulties by being marked by high frequency of conflict or aggression, difficulty with conflict resolution or bullying, although these children may be relatively skilled in joining or sustaining group activity (Lansford, Malone, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2010). By contrast, the group of children with ‘unsocialised’ conduct disorder would show a different pattern of peer interaction since they are usually more socially isolated with fewer opportunities for reciprocal play (Goodman & Scott, 2005). Children with severe conduct problems in the context of callous and unemotional traits may have a different socialisation pathway again (Frick et al., 2003, Jones et al., 2010); under-reactive and lacking in social inhibition. Little work has been done on observed peer interaction in this latter group, but theory would suggest observed episodes of cruelty or manipulation of others. Children with internalising disorders may also show social difficulties as a consequence of social withdrawal or social inhibition (Mesman, Bongers, & Koot, 2001). When this becomes pathological the child's lack of self-esteem and confidence may cause behavioural inhibition and avoidance of new challenges or fear engendering situations; affecting social interaction. Shyness is likely to be seen in observed difficulties in joining, sustaining and involvement in reciprocal play. These children are often seen as being aloof and vulnerable to bullying. They may spend more time alongside teachers and assistants (Goodman & Scott, 2005).

The presence of such a range of discrete social difficulties across psychopathology and disability implies an equivalent importance for the assessment of social functioning in clinical diagnosis and research (Banaschewski, 2010). Accurate assessments of social impairments may help to characterise disorders and differentiate their phenotypes; they may help understand routes into psychopathology and targets for treatment. Within such assessment, the naturalistic observation of social functioning, and in particular the structured observation of peer interaction, has a number of strengths (Pellegrini, 2001). Specific peer relationship difficulties have been identified across the range of socio-emotional, behavioural and communication disorders (Agam et al., 2003, Hartup, 2005, Guralnick et al., 2006). The free-for-all of the playground is a challenging social environment; children engaging in successful peer interaction there demonstrate social-cognitive and communication skills at a more sophisticated level than those evident in other school contexts (Pellegrini, 1998) and nascent social difficulties often first become apparent in that context. Furthermore, the social interaction observed in the playground can reflect key ‘real time’ social information processing that may differ in its mechanism from the more evaluative reasoning assessed in lab-based psychometric tests (Crick & Dodge, 1994). Observation of peer interaction in naturalistic settings is thus likely to be a particularly sensitive assessment context. It is a common clinical observation, for example, that children with high functioning autism can succeed in formal social understanding tasks in the structured environment of the clinic while showing major difficulties in social relating with peers in the community. Finally, observations in the school playground have ecological validity and are in a readymade, reasonably predictable, structured environment for observation. They can complement teacher and parent reports by avoiding reporting bias.

The Manchester Inventory for Playground Observation (MIPO) was designed with these considerations in mind; and in the tradition of systematic observational techniques combining the benefits of naturalistic methods with a commitment to generating valid, objective and reliable data (Salvia & Ysseldyke, 2004). The MIPO aims to be a rigorous, robust yet pragmatically usable observational schedule for social interaction on the playground, useful for both general clinical assessment and research purposes. To this end it was theoretically structured to enable the identification of different patterns of social impairment across disorder, on the assumption that the social impairment associated with different aetiological constructs and psychopathological entities will be manifest within different domains of peer social functioning. Accordingly the scale was constructed in four subscales, which reflect important aspects of peer socialisation for a range of developmental disorders (further details of individual items are outlined in the online Appendix A): A Pro-social Skills subscale codes for those higher-level social skills which children need to facilitate successful interaction. The choice of items has been influenced by developmental theories of peer interaction, which emphasize approaching, and successfully joining an interaction as important skills in peer relationships. A Conflict Management subscale codes reflect the extent of peer-conflict in which the child is involved, as well as the degree to which the child is able to successfully resolve such situations. Peer conflict is not uncommon on the primary school playground but is expected to be exaggerated in externalizing disorders where there may be amplified responses to minor conflicts due to impulsivity or attributional bias. A Confiding and Care-Seeking/Giving subscale reflects the distinction developmental theorists have drawn between peer interactions and friendships: The former referring to skills in joining and sustaining interactions as described in the Pro-social skills subscale while the latter is characterised by shared enjoyment of intimate reciprocal social exchange within a stable dyad (Howes, Rubin, Ross, & French, 1988). The subscale codes for the child's ability to appropriately give and receive care from peers (and adults who may be present in the playground) and to enjoy engagement in intimate confiding relationships. An Atypicality subscale codes for the presence of unusual behaviours particularly characteristic of Autistic Spectrum and related disorders, which directly impact on the child's ability to engage in successful peer interaction.

This current study reports on the reliability and validity of this observational method in the context of school playground observations. Two levels of discriminant validity test were undertaken: the first whether the instrument, despite its relatively short observational sampling period, could identify meaningful social impairment in clinical cases compared to age-matched class controls. Secondly, whether it could discriminate different profiles of social impairment between clinical diagnostic groupings along the lines predicted by theory. On the assumption that the instrument should be measuring trait impairment over at least the short term, we also examined test–retest stability to indicate whether variation in playground and activity type over time would affect the measure of the core construct. Criterion validity was examined against an existing instrument measuring a similar construct.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited from Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services (CAMHS) and Speech and Language Therapy (SLT) services in Greater Manchester, UK. Four Child Psychiatrists within two CAMHS teams made sequential unselected referrals of children with formal clinical diagnoses in groupings of Externalising Disorder (including Conduct Disorder, Oppositional Defiant Disorder, and ADHD), Internalising Disorder (including primary Depression and Anxiety disorders) and Autistic Spectrum

Sample

Descriptive characteristics of the clinical sample are presented in Table 1. In 43% of the sample there was one comorbid diagnosis and in 5.6% there were two or more comorbid diagnoses. Mean age was 8.8 years; ethnicity included 88% white British, 5.9%, Asian British, 5.9%, black British. The different clinical diagnostic groupings did not differ significantly in distribution of age or gender. Presence of co-occurring diagnoses was associated with more impaired scores on all sub-scales except

Discussion

Inter-observer reliability on the MIPO was good, with the mean values for the scale falling within the “substantial agreement” range (Landis & Koch, 1977). Eight of the individual MIPO items – ‘Joining’, ‘Sustaining Social Interaction’, ‘Social Reciprocity’, ‘Conflict Intensity’, ‘Conflict instigator’, ‘Atypicality’, ‘Aloofness’ and ‘Verbal Interaction’ – had κs greater than or equal to .8; falling within Landis and Koch's “almost perfect agreement” category (Landis & Koch, 1977). No item had a

Conclusion

The MIPO playground observation is practical to carry out in a typical school assessment visit and provides vivid data on real-time social functioning with peers. Because we have shown the short-term stability of a MIPO observation, the results could be considered indicative within an assessment, and provide a cost-effective complement to other domains of assessment such as history-taking, clinical examination, psychometric examination and laboratory based social cognition tasks, all of which

References (38)

  • N.R. Crick et al.

    A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • G. Dunn

    Statistics in psychiatry

    (1999)
  • P.J. Frick et al.

    Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2003)
  • R. Goodman et al.

    Child psychiatry

    (2005)
  • I.I. Gottesman et al.

    The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • Green, J.M. (2009). Attachment and social impairment in development. In Attachment: Current focus and future...
  • J.M. Green et al.

    Attachment disorganisation and psychopathology: New findings in attachment research and their potential implications for developmental psychopathology in childhood

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • F.M. Gresham et al.

    Social skills rating system manual

    (1990)
  • M.J. Guralnick et al.

    Stability, change, and correlates of peer relationships of young children with mild developmental delays

    Child Development

    (2006)
  • Cited by (29)

    • Do Tangible User Interfaces promote social behaviour during free play? A comparison of autistic and typically-developing children playing with passive and digital construction toys

      2019, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      From a positive psychology perspective, free play is characterised as freely chosen, self-directed, intrinsically motivated, rule-guided activity that is engaged in in non-stressful situations and states of being (Gray, 2013). Social free play may be particularly challenging for individuals with autism due to it requiring a high degree of behavioural flexibility and social awareness (Gibson et al., 2011). Studying free play with a construction toy is warranted because previous research has demonstrated that autistic children are more likely to engage in this construction play, given a free choice (Holmes & Willoughby, 2009).

    • Reliability and validity of the Pragmatics Observational Measure (POM): A new observational measure of pragmatic language for children

      2014, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      The psychometric properties of the POM need to be assessed in other clinical groups likely to have pragmatic language difficulties, including autism spectrum disorders, and a range of other communication, behavioural, cognitive and learning disorders. Future research could also look at the relationship between the POM and instruments that assess peer socialisation in naturalistic contexts such as the Manchester Inventory for Playground Observation (MIPO: Gibson, Hussain, Holsgrove, Adams, & Green, 2011). Considering all analyses, it can be concluded that the POM demonstrated good psychometric characteristics.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text