Self-regulation during pretend play in children with intellectual disability and in normally developing children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2007.05.003Get rights and content

Abstract

This study investigated the symbolic behavior and the self-regulation in dyads of children with intellectual disability and of normally developing children. Specifically, these processes were studied in link with the children's characteristics (mental age, linguistic level, individual pretend play level). The sample included 80 participants, 40 children with intellectual disability and 40 normally developing children, matched according to their mental age, ranged from 3 to 6 years old. First, a developmental assessment was performed (about cognitive, language and pretend play dimensions); then, in peers dyads, the children were elicited to pretend play by means of four kinds of material referring to four types of scripts (tea party, doctor, transportation, substitute objects eliciting creativity). The average symbolic behavior in individual and dyadic play contexts did not differ in both groups, but the average self-regulation in the group with intellectual disability was lower than in the normally developing group. Some positive partial correlations were obtained between mental age, language abilities, individual pretend play, dyadic pretend play and several self-regulatory strategies in both groups although they varied in importance between groups. Clusters analyses showed that individual and dyadic pretend play explained self-regulation in children of both groups. Specifically, in both groups, the higher was symbolic behavior in creativity context, the higher was self-regulation.

Introduction

This study explored the symbolic behavior and self-regulation in children with intellectual disability in dyadic pretend play situations. Specifically, the impact of children's characteristics (mental age, presence or absence of intellectual disability, linguistic level, individual pretend play level) on their levels in symbolic behavior in dyads and on their level in their self-regulatory strategies was examined. Our research is based on an integrated model of self-regulation that defines this process as following: to attain an identified objective, the person plans, explores the means at his or her disposal, maintains his or her attention and motivation during the problem solving, evaluates and adjusts his or her actions and when necessary, solicits the social environment by asking for help, joint attention or approval. The self-regulatory strategies were transposed into categories of observable indicatives in order to analyze the child's verbal and non-verbal behavior (Nader-Grosbois, Normandeau, Ricard, & Quintal, in press).

Self-regulation abilities develop more accurately during preschool period, it represents a significant cognitive-developmental hallmarks and an important achievement associated with social, behavioral and academic competence (Bronson, 2000, Flavell, 1977; Gilmore, Cuskelly, & Hayes, 2003; Kopp, 1982, Pressley, 1995). Preschool children are more and more capable of true inner self-regulation, in using rules, strategies and plans to guide their behavior (Kopp, 1982). Moreover, in this period, they become more and more interested by mastery challenges, they are more and more able to solve problems and they control task progression more efficiently. Several authors took an interest in this development during infancy and childhood (Chang & Burns, 2005; De La Ossa & Gauvain, 2001; Gardner & Rogoff, 1990; Hudson, Shapiro, & Sosa, 1995; Nelson-Legall, 1987, Puustinen, 1998; Sethi, Mischel, Aber, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 2000; Stipek, Recchia, & McClintic, 1992; St-Laurent & Moss, 2002; Winnykamen, 1993). According to Bronson (2000), even if the self-regulation evolves with age, there are also individual differences in this development in children at the same age. So, in our study, we hypothesized a positive link between mental age and self-regulation level in normally developing children and in children with intellectual disability. Cuskelly, Zhang, and Gilmore (1998) considered the self-regulation to be one of the key features of development in children with Down syndrome as in typically developing children. But some authors pointed the self-regulation as an overall deficient process in persons with intellectual disabilities (Whitman, 1990) and described them as being dependent and having an external locus of control (Zigler & Balla, 1982); because of their inability to effectively self-regulate and the negative consequences associated with failure, they usually look to others for assistance (Whitman, 1990). These persons cannot cope with a new situation and they experience difficulty in transferring their knowledge. According to Shapiro (1986), these difficulties can be due to the low expectations of the society about their abilities. He suggested that they can be taught to manage their own behavior. Lanfaloni, Baglioni, and Tafi (1997) explained the limitations in these persons’ adaptive behavior by an inappropriate development of regulators. As Blanc et al. (2000) observed, the dysregulation of activity is probably a general cognitive disorder which is implied in infantile autism and in intellectual disability. Glenn and Cunningham (2002) reported that, in individuals with Down syndrome, self-regulation development is delayed, as in other developmental areas: the pattern of sequence is maintained but the rhythm is slower and the plateaus are more extended. Other authors identified some specific deficiencies in self-regulation in children with intellectual disability, as following: particular difficulty in planning, in monitoring and in flexibility in problem solving (Gilmore et al., 2003; Goodman, Fox, & Glutting, 1986; Kopp, Krakow, & Johnson, 1983); in self-regulated attention (Berry, Gunn, & Andrews, 1984; Goodman, 1979, Kopp, 1990, Kopp et al., 1983; Landry & Chapieski, 1989; Paparella & Kasari, 2004); difficulties in request (Fidler, Philofsky, Hepburn, & Rogers, 2005; Mundy, Kasari, Sigman, & Ruskin, 1995); poor task persistence and high help elicitation during task achievement, in using objects and finally, low self-motivation (Diener & Dweck, 1978; Glenn & Cunningham, 2002; Ruskin, Kasari, Mundy, & Sigman, 1994a). In our study, we postulated some specific deficiencies in self-regulatory strategies in children with intellectual disability such as self-attention, self-motivation and planning in dyadic pretend play situations.

To elicit this self-regulation, different contexts can be put in place toward the children. Pretend play constitutes an original context for self-regulation assessment since learning situations are the most used context in previous studies. Pretend play appears in the second year of life. Its development follows a gradual and quite predictable trajectory, characterized by more and more complex levels. Three underlying mechanisms support its development: decentration, decontextualisation and integration (Hughes, 1999). Neopiagetians (Bates, Benigni, Bretherton, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1979; Bretherton, 1984, Nicolich, 1977) proposed a categorization of the symbolic behaviors according to different dimensions. Bretherton (1984) analyzed pretend play evolution according to the structural components of the script: the roles a child plays, the actions he mimes and the objects he uses. Higginbotham and Baker (1981) considered four categories of social participation during pretend play, from solitary play to cooperative play. This theoretical background founded the elaboration of the coding grid for dyadic pretend play (in Appendix A), used in our study.

The preschool children's language level participates in the development of self-regulation: they begin to use speech as a mean to control action and thought; so, their language become an important mediator for self-regulation (Bronson, 2000). Several properties of language help to maintain control: language assists memory, emotional regulation, though, reflection, planning and building, rebuilding and recombining in memory to form new concepts, plans and solutions for problems (Bronson, 2000). Language is an important tool to learn (by means of verbal instructions) and to engage (by means of self-verbalisations) in self-regulation. Whitman (1990, p. 354) thinks that the role of language, in analyzing tasks, in developing and implementing strategy-selection, self-monitoring, self-evaluation, self-instruction, and self-reinforcement procedures, is self-evident. When speech is internalized (about 4 years of age), it plays an effective role in self-regulation and is used as an inner mode of mental organization to monitor behavior and to guide their own thought processes and actions (Luria, 1961, Vygotsky, 1962). Socio-developmental theorists explained that children become progressively less managed by external verbal directives and more by their self-verbalisations that are gradually internalized. According to Whitman (1990), the individuals with intellectual disability, because of their extensive language deficiencies, are delayed in developing self-regulatory control; they have difficulty acquiring and improving their knowledge, to convey what they know and to learn to use self-instruction. So in our study, we postulated a positive link between linguistic level and self-regulation level in normally developing children and in children with intellectual disability.

Moreover, according to Vygotsky (1962), pretend play and imagination are particularly important in children's development because this skill allows them to specify their own challenges, objectives and actions toward their goals. During pretend play episodes, children follow social rules, without external control and they use usually speech (Kraft & Berk, 1998). However, few studies have investigated the link between pretend play and self-regulation. The influence of pretend play level on self-regulation abilities of children in other contexts (clean-up, problem solving) has been studied (Elias & Berk, 2002; Rosen, 1974). It has been shown that experiences of pretend play during infancy contribute on a major part to the development of self-regulation. When the child is playing, he actively organizes stimuli into models inducting comprehension of social norms and (s)he regulates one's behavior according to these norms (Elias & Berk, 2002). Sylva (1976) considered the contribution of pretend play in problem solving. Other authors focalised on self-regulation within the framework of pretend play (Garvey, 1977; Kraft & Berk, 1998; Mead, 1934). Blanc et al. (2000) compared the regulation of symbolic activity in children with autism and in children with intellectual disability. They supposed that the dysregulation of activity affects the pattern of mental representation (as shown by the specific disorder of pretend play in infantile autism) and in the same way the development of communication which, to be effective, requires the activation of consistent and regulated mental representations. They obtained significant negative relations between scores in pretend play and scores in dysregulation in children with intellectual disability; so, their regulation deficit interfered with the pretend play function. In our study, we postulated in both groups of children: firstly, a positive link between total scores in individual pretend play, in dyadic pretend play, and overall self-regulation; secondly, the individual and dyadic pretend play levels can contribute to the variability of the self-regulation level. We also hypothesized that the relations should vary in importance between specific self-regulation strategies (planning of one's action, evaluation of effects of one's action, use of one's resources, management of one's attention and motivation) and specific pretend play components (involvement, roles, actions, objects, social participation).

Section snippets

Participants

Our sample includes 80 participants, 40 children with intellectual disability (from special education classes) and 40 normally developing children (from normal nursery classes). The group with intellectual disability had a chronological age from 82 to 157 months (M = 115.03; S.D. = 24.72) and a mental age from 34 to 82 months (M = 55.45; S.D. = 12.03). The etiologies of intellectual disability ranged from genetic syndromes, to non-identifiable etiologies, and non-specific retardations due to

Participant's characteristics of development

Table 1 presented respective mean scores in language, individual pretend play and in dyadic pretend play of both groups.

The average language score did not significantly differ between groups, t(77) = .818, ns; nor did their average receptive language score, t(77) = −.423, ns; or their productive language score, t(77) = .451, ns. Even if the average language score in repetition did not differ significantly between the groups it approximated the signification threshold, t(77) = 1.96, p < .06.

Controlling

Discussion

Firstly, in children with intellectual disability, when their mental age increased, their overall self-regulation improved as well as their self-regulation in tea time scenario, their self-attention and their self-motivation. In normally developing children, when their mental age increased, their self-regulation in different scenarios (tea time, transportation, creativity) and several self-regulatory strategies (objective, exploration of means or planning, joint attention, request, attention,

References (71)

  • M.B. Bronson

    Self-regulation in early childhood. Nature and nurture

    (2000)
  • B. Céleste et al.
    (1997)
  • F. Chang et al.

    Attention in preschoolers: Associations with effortful control and motivation

    Child Development

    (2005)
  • M. Cuskelly et al.

    The importance of self-regulation in young children with Down syndrome

    International Journal of Disability, Development and Education

    (1998)
  • J.L. De La Ossa et al.

    Joint attention by mothers and children while using plans

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2001)
  • C.I. Diener et al.

    An analysis of learned helplessness II: The processing of success

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1978)
  • D.J. Fidler et al.

    Nonverbal requesting and problem-solving by toddlers with Down syndrome

    American Journal on Intellectual Disability

    (2005)
  • J.H. Flavell

    Cognitive development

    (1977)
  • K. Fuson

    The development of self-regulating aspects of speech: A review

  • W. Gardner et al.

    Children's deliberateness of planning according to task circumstances

    Developmental Psychology

    (1990)
  • C. Garvey

    Play

    (1977)
  • S. Glenn et al.

    Self-regulation in children and young people with Down syndrome

  • J.F. Goodman

    The Lock Box: An instrument to evaluate mental organization in preschool children

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (1979)
  • J.F. Goodman et al.

    Contributions of the Lock Box to preschool assessment

    Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment

    (1986)
  • P. Gunn et al.

    Looking behaviour of Down's syndrome infants

    American Journal of Mental Deficiency

    (1982)
  • M.C. Haelewyck et al.

    L’autorégulation: une des portes d’entrée vers l’autodétermination des personnes à incapacités intellectuelles?

    Revue Francophone de la déficience Intellectuelle

    (2004)
  • D.J. Higginbotham et al.

    Social participation and cognitive differences in hearing-impaired and normally hearing preschoolers

    Volta Review

    (1981)
  • J.A. Hudson et al.

    Planning in the real world: Preschool children's scripts and plans for familiar events

    Child Development

    (1995)
  • F.P. Hughes

    Children, play and development

    (1999)
  • O.H.M. Jones

    Prelinguistic communication skills in Down's Syndrome and normal infants

  • C. Kasari et al.

    Attention regulation by children with Down Syndrome: Coordinated joint attention and social referencing looks

    American Journal on Mental Retardation

    (1995)
  • C. Kasari et al.

    Affect and attention in children with Down Syndrome

    American Journal on Intellectual Disability

    (1990)
  • A. Khomsi

    ELO, Evaluation du language oral

    (2001)
  • C.B. Kopp

    Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective

    Developmental Psychology

    (1982)
  • C.B. Kopp

    The growth of self-monitoring among young children with Down syndrome

  • Cited by (36)

    • Time spent playing predicts early reading and math skills through associations with self-regulation

      2022, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although these studies establish a link between specific characteristics of play and self-regulation, none measure time spent engaged in play naturalistically, in the context of children's daily lives. Instead, they use experimentally induced play tasks and measure play quality, and thus the ecological validity of results is questionable when it comes to making generalizations to the play experiences that children in which engage in their everyday lives (e.g., Schmitt et al., 2018; Slot et al., 2017; Vieillevoye & Nader-Grosbois, 2008; Whitebread, Coltman, Jameson, & Lander, 2009). This study adds to the current literature by bridging the gap between time diary studies that measure children's naturalistic, real-world play but do not contain rich measurement on their developmental outcomes and developmental studies focused on specific characteristics of play but reveal little about whether unstructured play opportunities for children have positive associations with their development.

    • A multidimensional evaluation of the benefits of an ecologically realistic training based on pretend play for preschoolers’ cognitive control and self-regulation: From behavior to the underlying theta neuro-oscillatory activity

      2022, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, the improvement in the children’s play maturity did not benefit children’s self-regulation in their daily activities, nor did it influence cognitive control and MFT oscillatory activity. A higher level of play maturity is associated with increased performance in cognitive control (Carlson et al., 2014; Slot et al., 2017; Vieillevoye & Nader-Grosbois, 2008), but there is little evidence that pretend play is effective for training preschoolers’ cognitive control. Only two studies have shown that regularly engaging children in pretend play increases their inhibition and set-shifting performance (Thibodeau et al., 2016; Traverso et al., 2015).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text