“Communicative skills in Spanish children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Analysis through parents’ perceptions and narrative production”

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2018.02.006Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The present study examined the communicative profile of children with ASD and children with ADHD.

  • Communicative deficits were not as severe in the group with ADHD as in the group with ASD.

  • Stereotyped language, non-verbal communication and the use of context were more affected in children with ASD than in ADHD.

  • The narrative analysis revealed that participants with ASD make more erroneous interpretations than participants with ADHD.

  • Communicative competence analysis has implications for optimizing the assessment process and planning the treatment.

Abstract

Background

Communicative skills are one of the main deficits experienced by children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). The differential diagnosis is a complex issue that clinicians often face. For this reason, this study has two objectives: 1) to analyze the similarities and differences that children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and children with ASD present in their linguistic and communicative skills, through parentś perceptions and narrative production; 2) to identify the relative importance of the skills analyzed in discriminating children with ASD versus children with ADHD, as well as the classification power of each of the two measurement methods used.

Method

Participants were 37 children with typical development (TD), 52 with ASD, and 35 with ADHD. Their communicative competence was assessed through The Children's Communication Checklist Second Edition (CCC-2) (Bishop, 2003) and a narrative task.

Results

The results of the family ratings indicated that the difficulties were not as severe in ADHD as in ASD, where the deficit was much more pronounced in the interpretation of the context, non-verbal communication, and stereotyped language. On the narrative task, both groups displayed difficulties on expressing relevant information and inferential comprehension, and they differed on erroneous interpretations, with the children with ASD presenting worse performance. These variables showed a greater capacity to classify participants in the diagnostic categories studied.

Conclusions

These findings have direct clinical implications for optimizing the assessment process, making it possible to identify which specific language aspects can best differentiate between groups.

Introduction

Recent studies have highlighted the diagnostic conundrum that clinicians often face when establishing differential diagnoses of verbally fluent school-age children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (Grzadzinski, Dick, Lord, & Bishop, 2016). These two neurodevepmental disorders show a high co-occurrence; in fact, there is an overlap in genetic, neurobiological and neuropsychological bases, such as executive functioning or theory of mind skills (Miranda, Baixauli, Colomer & Roselló, 2013). One relevant aspect that can shed light on this complex topic is the analysis of communication. Persistent deficits in social communication are present across the autism spectrum (Schuh, Eigsti, & Mirman, 2016) and, even if they are not included in the core diagnostic criteria, language and pragmatic disturbances may often occur in ADHD (Bellani, Moretti, Perlini, & Brambilla, 2011; Green, Johnson, & Bretherton, 2014).

Studies that have compared the communicative competence in ADHD and in ASD (specifically, high functioning autism, without language impairment) have mainly used the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC-2) (Bishop, 2003), designed to assess social-communicative impairments in daily interactions. Most research has found similar deficits in both populations on coherence and inappropriate initiations. However, they do not seem to be as severe in ADHD as in ASD, where the impairment is more pronounced in the interpretation of the context, non-verbal communication, and stereotyped language (Geurts et al., 2004; Geurts & Embrecths, 2008; Helland, Biringer, Helland, & Heimann, 2012; Kuijper, Hartman, Bogaerds-Hazenberg, & Hendriks, 2017).

One of the main dimensions of communicative competence is narrative ability, which has proven to be a vulnerable domain in both ASD and ADHD. A meta-analysis about this topic in children with ASD found significant deficits in macrostructure (coherence and cohesive adequacy), microstructure (productivity and grammar) and internal state language (Baixauli, Colomer, Roselló, & Miranda, 2016). Children with ADHD experience difficulties in the elaboration and comprehension of stories as well, which is probably related to the cognitive characteristics of the disorder (Green et al., 2014; Ygual, Roselló, & Miranda, 2010). Despite these evidences, to the best of our knowledge, only two studies have tried to perform a comparative analysis of the narrative discourse produced by individuals with ADHD and with ASD, both using Tuesday (Wiesner, 1998), a story included in the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 2000). First, Rumpf, Kamp-Becker, Becker, and Kauschke (2012) found a similar performance in children with ASD, ADHD and typical development (TD) on aspects such as variety of syntactic structures, mean utterance length and use of connectors. However, the groups with ASD and ADHD produced significantly shorter and less coherent narrations than the TD group. Moreover, the group with ASD produced significantly fewer pronominal references and cognitive internal state terms compared to the group with ADHD and TD. More recently, Kuijper et al. (2017) found similarities between the groups with ADHD, ASD and TD, regarding story length. Similar to Rumpf et al.’s (2012) findings, children with ADHD and ASD showed difficulties in producing a coherent narrative. However, participants with ASD had more limitations regarding syntactic complexity and children with ADHD had more problems in their choice of referring expressions and speech fluency. The study by Kuijper et al. (2017) went one step further by analyzing the associations between the CCC-2 and narrative skills, obtaining a moderate correlation. In addition, significant associations were also found between narrative ability, theory of mind and working memory in both clinical groups.

From our point of view, the CCC-2 and the narrative task offer complementary but different information. The analysis of the narrative ability provides very relevant data, such as quantitative indicators about language samples. Although narratives involve a degree of pragmatic skill, they are more formal than everyday conversations and they have been shown predictive capacity of academic achievement (Griffin, Hemphill, Camp, & Wolf, 2004). This information would be difficult to obtain from questionnaires fulfilled by parents and teachers, that assess everyday communication and might give a more representative account of the child’s behavior in everyday situations.

In summary, little research has been conducted that directly compare abilities across neurodevelopmental disorders within the same study and how different tasks demands might elicit different profiles (Botting, Bean-Ellawadi & Williams, 2016). In this sense, it might be useful to explore the utility of narrative measures which are administered by trained clinicians versus questionnaire measures based on parent or teacher ratings. Specifically, the identification of possible specific communication impairments across ADHD and ASD through different assessment procedures may help in the consideration of more appropriate intervention objectives. Consequently, the present study proposes the following objectives:

  • To analyze the similarities and differences in the linguistic and communicative skills of children with ADHD, ASD, and TD, assessed using two different methods: parent ratings on the CCC-2 and a narrative task.

  • To identify the relative importance of the CCC-2 subscales and the narrative task indicators in discriminating between children with ASD versus children with ADHD, as well as the classification power of each assessment method.

Our study extends previous research regarding pragmatic and narrative ability in ASD and ADHD. An important issue refers to the method used to elicit narratives. As stated above, both studies by Rumpf et al. (2012) and Kuijper et al. (2017), used the story book Tuesday (Wiesner, 1991) included during the assessment through the ADOS (Lord et al., 2000). Following the ADOS guidelines, the experimenter introduces the narrative and then the child continues until he is interrupted by the examiner, who finishes the story. Consequently, the child does not need to provide the initial setting or the final resolution of the story. In this way the assessment of the ability to elaborate a complete narrative might be left out. Moreover, comprehension was not assessed in previous studies.

Other contribution of the present study is the use of discriminant analysis in order to identify the relative importance of the CCC-2 subscales and the narrative task indicators in discriminating between children with ASD versus children with ADHD, as well as the classification power of each assessment method. These objectives are not considered in earlier work.

Furthermore, the cultural and linguistic context in which the study is developed is different from that of previous research, which have been carried out in other countries. Cultural issues are crucial in understanding neurodevelopmental disorders (Norbury & Sparks, 2013). This consideration is even more noteworthy if we take into account that pragmatic language abilities are highly susceptible to cultural and linguistic variation. Aspects such as turn taking, use of humor, eye contact and other nonverbal strategies for maintaining interaction are largely determined by cultural rules. In particular, in Western, English-speaking cultures, people tend to value complex narratives with a clear temporal and causal structure. In contrast, in Hispanic culture, a stronger focus is placed on the emotional importance of the events (Norbury & Sparks, 2013, Sparks, 2008). Given that different languages and cultures might organize social interactions differently, cross-linguistic studies are crucially relevant. Therefore, when assessing language and communication, it is important to consider the impact of language specific features on the manifestation of neurodevelopmental disorders (Leonard, 2009).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants in this study were 124 children between 7 and 11 years old. They had an intelligence quotient (IQ) equal to or above 80, measured by the K-BIT (Kaufman & Kaufman 2000), and were distributed in three groups: 37 children with TD, 52 children with ASD (severity level 1, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- Fifth Edition, (DSM-5, APA, 2013), and 35 children with ADHD, matched on age and IQ. Of them, 83.1% of the participants were boys, and 16.9% were

Statistical analyses

The statistical analyses were performed with the Statistical Package for the Social Science (SPSS) version 22.00 software. Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine the distribution of the variables using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test; those variables that did not show a normal distribution were transformed using square-root transformation.

To analyze the differences between the groups with TD, ASD, and ADHD on the variables selected from the CCC-2 and the variables from the narrative task,

Discussion

The first objective of this study was to assess the profile of alterations presented by children with ADHD and children with ASD in their linguistic and communicative abilities, evaluated, on the one hand, by the parent ratings on the CCC-2 questionnaire and, on the other, through the narration of a story.

Regarding parent ratings on the CCC-2, the results obtained on the different subscales on the general communication composite (GCC) and on the non-standardized pragmatic composite (PC)

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgement

This work is supported by the Spanish project PSI2016-78109 (AEI/FEDER, UE) and the predoctoral fellowship UV-INV-PREDOC15-265889.

References (56)

  • D.V.M. Bishop et al.

    Parent and Teacher report of pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children’s Communication Checklist in a clinical setting

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (2001)
  • D.V.M. Bishop et al.

    Identifying language impairment in children: Combining language test scores with parental report

    International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders

    (2009)
  • D.V.M. Bishop

    The children’s communication checklist (CCC-2)

    (2003)
  • E. Bora et al.

    Meta-analysis of social cognition in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): comparison with healthy controls and autistic spectrum disorder

    Psychological Medicine

    (2016)
  • N. Botting et al.

    Language impairments in childhood. A range of profiles: A variety of reasons

    Autism and Developmental Language Impairments

    (2016)
  • N. Crespo-Eguílaz et al.

    La adaptación al castellano de la Children’s Communication Checklist permite detectar las dificultades en el uso pragmático del lenguaje y diferenciar subtipos clínicos

    Revista De Neurología

    (2016)
  • M. Dennis et al.

    Inferential language in high-function children with autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2001)
  • J. Gazella et al.

    Children’s story retelling under different modality and task conditions: Implications for standardizing language sampling procedures?

    American Journal of Speech Language Pathology

    (2003)
  • H.M. Geurts et al.

    Language profiles in ASF SLI, and ADHD

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2008)
  • H.M. Geurts et al.

    Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls?

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • B. Green et al.

    Pragmatic language difficulties in children with hyperactivity and attention problems: An integrated review

    International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders

    (2014)
  • T.M. Griffin et al.

    Oral discourse in the preschool years and later literacy skills

    First Language

    (2004)
  • R. Grzadzinski et al.

    Parent-reported and clinician-observed autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): implications for practice under DSM-5?

    Molecular Autism

    (2016)
  • F. Happé et al.

    The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2006)
  • W.A. Helland et al.

    Exploring language profiles for children with ADHD and children with Asperger Syndrome

    Journal of Attention Disorders

    (2012)
  • W.A. Helland et al.

    Language profiles and mental health problems in children with specific language impairment and children with ADHD

    Journal of Attention Disorders

    (2014)
  • A.S. Kaufman et al.

    K-BIT. Test Breve de Inteligencia de Kauffman

    (2000)
  • P.R. Kinnear et al.

    SPSS for windows made simple

    (2000)
  • View full text