“You’re telling me!” The prevalence and predictors of pronoun reversals in children with autism spectrum disorders and typical development

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.03.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The rate of pronoun reversal in children with ASD is under 10%.

  • Children with ASD nonetheless produced more reversals than TD children.

  • Higher early language and joint attention scores predicted fewer reversals.

  • Reversals occur when language and social abilities develop asynchronously.

Abstract

Social and linguistic explanations have been proposed for pronoun reversals in young typically developing (TD) children and those with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The current study breaks new ground in investigating both explanations, comparing 18 TD toddlers and 15 children with ASD at similar language levels. Spontaneous speech was sampled every four months for six visits. Vocabulary and joint attention were also measured. Both groups produced pronoun reversals at low rates. The ASD group produced somewhat more reversals than the TD group, overall and at multiple visits. In the ASD group, early language and joint attention scores contributed significantly and independently to the incidence of reversal. Both linguistic and social factors seem implicated; moreover, reversals seem to occur when children’s language and social abilities develop asynchronously. These findings can help clinicians devise both linguistic and social interventions for the relevant children.

Introduction

Researchers of typical and atypical child development have become increasingly interested in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) over the past 20 years because of this disorder’s intriguing constellation of impairments. Social deficits are the most obvious, and indeed a core of the diagnosis (American Psychiatric Association, 2014), but combinations of motor, cognitive, and language impairments are also attested (Fein, 2011). One language impairment – consistently reported to be “common” in children with ASD – is the phenomenon of pronoun reversals; i.e., using ‘I’ for ‘you’ and/or ‘you’ for ‘I’ (e.g., a child saying ‘You want some more milk’ meaning ‘I want some more milk’). This impairment is mentioned in almost every description of language and communication in children with ASD, from the earliest descriptions targeted for clinical audiences (Kanner, 1946; Fay, 1971; Rutter, 1979) to later (Roberts, 1989, Roberts, 2014; McCann, Peppe, Gibbon, O’Hare & Rutherford, 2008; McGregor, Nunez, Cebula, & Gomez, 2008; Paul & Wilson, 2009; Klinger, O’Kelley, & Mussey, 2009; Wetherby, 2006; Tager-Flusberg, Paul, & Lord, 2005; Luyster & Lord, 2009), and has emerged recently in textbooks designed for general child development audiences (Levine and Munsch, 2014, Rathus, 2014). However, the origins – and indeed, actual prevalence – of this error in children with ASD, are still unclear.

Successful pronominal reference requires the mastery of a sophisticated grammatical paradigm, involving number (‘I’ vs. ‘we’), person (‘I’ vs. ‘you’ vs. ‘she’), and case (‘I’ vs. ‘me’), amongst other factors. Reversal errors involve the person component, such that ‘I’ and ‘you’ become confused. These errors are attested in TD toddlers’ spontaneous speech, albeit at varying rates; pronouns typically emerge around 1.5 years of age and reversals are most common before 2.5 years (Chiat, 1982, Clark, 1978, Charney, 1980, Dale and Crain-Thoreson, 1993, Evans and Demuth, 2012, Loveland and Landry, 1986, Oshima-Takane, 1992; Oshima-Takane, Goodz, & Derensky, 1996), and have been hypothesized to derive at least partially, from linguistic immaturity. That is, before children have worked out the paradigm, personal pronouns may be subject to language processing constraints, and/or used as frozen forms within specific discourse contexts (Dale & Crain-Thoreson, 1993; Lee, Hobson & Chiat, 1994). As children develop better language skills, they are able to access the pronoun paradigm with greater facility, and the reversals cease. On this account, a higher rate of pronoun reversals in children with ASD may be attributable to their delayed onset of language (Tager-Flusberg et al., 1990; Tek, Mesite, Fein, & Naigles, 2014), and/or subtle deficits in their use of specific grammatical forms (Eigsti, Bennetto, & Dadlani, 2007; Tager-Flusberg, 2006). Across multiple studies, both TD children and children with ASD have been reported to use more pronoun reversals in imitative contexts (Carpenter, Tomasello & Striano, 2005; Dale and Crain-Thoreson, 1993, Evans and Demuth, 2012, Roberts, 1989, Tager-Flusberg et al., 2005), suggesting that these pronouns were not yet linguistically productive (i.e., integrated into the grammatical system).

Appropriate use of first and second person (personal) pronouns also involves social factors such as perspective-taking, in that speakers use ‘I’ for self-reference and ‘you’ for addressee reference, and the actual referents of ‘I’ and ‘you’ change with each speaker (Chiat, 1982; Clark, 1978; Hobson, Garcia-Perez, & Lee, 2010; Ricard, Girouard, & Gouin-Decarie, 1999). Because children with ASD frequently have difficulties with perspective-taking (or deixis) (Loveland, 1984), it has often been proposed that the use of pronoun reversals in this population is attributable to their social impairments. However, the pervasiveness of these social impairments in children with ASD sometimes seems at odds with actual pronoun reversal rates in this population. That is, Tager-Flusberg’s (1994) analysis of the conversations of six boys with ASD revealed a pronoun reversal rate of about 13%. Tager-Flusberg reported that pronoun reversals were more frequent in these boys than in a language-matched group of children with Down syndrome, but did not present a comparison with typically developing (TD) children. A recent investigation of school-age deaf children with ASD, using ASL as their primary language, reported no reversals at all among the children who used pronouns (Shield, Meier, & Tager-Flusberg, 2015). While elicited production tasks have found that school-age children with ASD produce more pronoun reversals than mental-age-matched TD children, researchers have also reported that the most common error in the ASD group was pronoun avoidance, i.e., using proper names instead. Furthermore, comprehension tasks with this age group have yielded close-to-ceiling performance in differentiating ‘you’ and ‘me’ when asked (Jordan, 1989; Lee et al., 1994). In sum, the ‘common-ness’ of pronoun reversals in the language of children with ASD cannot be assumed, either in absolute terms or in relation to TD children.

A recent case study of two toddlers suggests a somewhat different interpretation on how both social and linguistic factors may be operating in the incidence and characteristics of pronoun reversal (Evans & Demuth, 2012). These toddlers were precocious, beginning word production at 11 months and personal pronoun use at 13–16 months. Notably, both children reversed first and second person pronouns at much higher rates than usually reported for TD children (80–95%, cf. Dale & Crain-Thoreson, 1993), and one was diagnosed with Asperger’s Syndrome at 5 years of age. The authors propose that the children’s linguistic precocity and high pronoun reversal rate were connected, such that they had begun to acquire the pronominal paradigm before their social capacities grasped its deictic (perspective-taking) nature. On this account, then, more frequent pronoun reversals in children with ASD may be attributed to an asynchrony between their levels of language and social development.

What no one has yet studied is how linguistic and social factors might jointly contribute to pronoun reversal in children with ASD. The current paper, then, has three goals. First, we assess the prevalence of pronoun reversals in a larger sample of children with ASD (n = 15) than has previously been studied, and compare this prevalence with that of 18 TD toddlers at similar language levels. Second, we assess the children’s levels of social impairment in relation to their general language levels. An early index of these social impairments is the well-documented difficulty that children with ASD have with initiating and sustaining joint attention (JA) during dyadic interactions (Luyster, Kadlec, Connolly, Carter, & Tager-Flusberg, 2008; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990; Paul, Chawarska, Cicchetti, & Volkmar, 2008; Rollins and Snow, 1998, Siller and Sigman, 2008). We investigate whether the proposed conditions for pronoun reversal – a developmental asynchrony between social and language development – are found in our sample, such that children with ASD who are matched with TD children on language nonetheless fall below the TD children’s levels of JA. Third, we directly assess the relative contributions of children’s social/perspective taking (i.e., JA) and language skills to their early vs. later incidence of pronoun reversals.

Section snippets

Participants

The participants for this study included 18 TD children and 15 children diagnosed with ASD, which was confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000). The children with ASD were recruited via service providers in the northeastern U.S.; all were verbal and were receiving at least five hours of Applied Behavioral Analysis therapy per week (ABA; Lovaas, 1987). The participants were visited every 4 months for 6 visits; at visit 1, the TD children averaged 20.5

Results

The means and standard deviations for each pronoun measure are presented in Table 2 for both groups. Number of pronouns increased with age for both groups, and was subjected to a 2-way repeated measures ANOVA with group (TD, ASD) as the between-subjects variable and visit (early, late) as the within-subjects variable. A significant main effect of visit was found [F(1.30) = 74.790, p < .001, ηp2 = .714] with children producing fewer pronouns at earlier visits than later visits. No main effect for

Discussion

This study investigated the prevalence and predictors of children’s pronoun reversals during interactions with their caregivers over a span of 2 years. There were three major findings: First, pronoun reversal proportions (out of all pronouns produced) were quite low at all visits for both TD and ASD groups. The highest mean prevalence recorded was 7.07% for the ASD group at visit 1. Second, three significant group effects emerged: The ASD group’s proportion of pronoun reversals was

Conflict of interests

None of the authors have any conflicts of interest involving this research.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by a National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders grant to L. Naigles (Grant number: R01 DC007428). We extend our gratitude to Rose Jaffery, Janina Piotroski, and Andrea Tovar for assistance in data collection, to Emily Potrzeba and Sabrine Elberkani for their assistance with coding of the pronouns, and to the undergraduates of the UConn Child Language Lab for transcribing the play sessions and performing reliability coding of JA. We appreciate the

References (63)

  • H. Tager-Flusberg

    Defining language phenotypes in autism

    Clinical Neuroscience Research

    (2006)
  • S. Özçalışkan et al.

    Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism

    Autism

    (2015)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2014)
  • L. Bartak et al.

    The use of personal pronouns by autistic children

    Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia

    (1974)
  • E. Bates et al.

    Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development

  • M. Carpenter et al.

    Role reversal imitation, and language in typically developing infants and children with autism

    Infancy

    (2005)
  • R. Charney

    Speech roles and the development of personal pronouns

    Journal of Child Language

    (1980)
  • S. Chiat

    If I were you and you were me-the analysis of pronouns in a pronoun- reversing child

    Journal of Child Language

    (1982)
  • E. Clark

    From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis in language acquisition

  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • P.S. Dale et al.

    Pronoun reversals: Who, when, and why?

    Journal of Child Language

    (1993)
  • I.M. Eigsti et al.

    Beyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2007)
  • K.E. Evans et al.

    Individual differences in pronoun reversal: evidence from two longitudinal case studies

    Journal of Child Language

    (2012)
  • W.H. Fay

    On normal and autistic pronouns

    Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders

    (1971)
  • L. Fenson et al.

    Variability in early communicative development

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1994)
  • A. Goodwin et al.

    Comprehension of wh-questions precedes their production in typical development and autism spectrum disorders

    Autism Research

    (2012)
  • R.P. Hobson et al.

    Person-centred (Deictic) expressions and autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2010)
  • R.R. Jordan

    An experimental comparison of the understanding and use of speaker-addressee personal pronouns in autistic children

    British Journal of Disorders of Communication

    (1989)
  • L.I. Kanner

    Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1946)
  • C. Kasari et al.

    Language outcome in autism: randomized comparison of joint attention and play interventions

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2008)
  • L. Klinger et al.

    Assessment of intellectual functioning in autism spectrum disorders

  • A. Lee et al.

    I, you, me, and autism: an experimental study

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1994)
  • L. Levine et al.

    Child development: an active learning approach

    (2014)
  • C. Lord et al.

    The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2000)
  • I.O. Lovaas

    Behavioral treatment and normal educational and intellectual functioning in young autistic children

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1987)
  • K. Loveland et al.

    Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1986)
  • K.A. Loveland

    Learning about points of view- spatial perspective and the acquisition of ‘I/you'

    Journal of Child Language

    (1984)
  • R. Luyster et al.

    Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2008)
  • R. Luyster et al.

    Language in autism

  • B. MacWhinney

    The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk

    (1995)
  • J. McCann et al.

    The prosody-language relationship in children with high-functioning autism

  • Cited by (59)

    • Ad-hoc and scalar implicatures in children with autism spectrum disorder

      2021, Journal of Communication Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Pragmatics is a core weakness in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), with persistent impairments both in receptive and in expressive language (DSM-5: American Psychiatric Association, 2013; ICD-10: World Health Organization, 2018). Individuals with ASD show difficulties in several areas of pragmatics, including topic initiation, maintenance and change, comprehension of irony, metaphors and humor, turn taking, register selection; use of pronouns and deictic terms and detection of violations of conversational maxims (e.g., Baltaxe, 1977; Chin & Bernard-Opitz, 2000; Clifford & Dissanayake, 2008; Happé, 1995a; Loukusa et al., 2007; Melogno, Pinto, & Levi, 2012; Naigles et al., 2016; Ozonoff & Miller, 1996; Reddy, Williams, & Vaughan, 2002; Rundblad & Annaz, 2010; Surian, Baron-Cohen, & Van der Lely, 1996; White and Nelson, 20,015). Most of these pragmatic skills require a speaker to go beyond the literal meaning of utterances or expressions and infer their pragmatic meaning.

    • How children acquire pronouns

      2023, The Routledge Handbook of Pronouns
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, United States.

    View full text