The relative importance of age and IQ as predictors of outcomes in Intensive Behavioral Intervention

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.06.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Outcomes are highly variable in Intensive Behavioral Intervention.

  • We studied initial age and IQ in predicting cognitive and adaptive outcomes.

  • IQ is a strong predictor but younger age is related to making large cognitive gains.

  • Children over age 8 and/or with very low IQ had uniformly poor outcomes.

  • Results strongly support the importance of early intervention.

Abstract

Outcomes of Intensive Behavioral Intervention are known to be highly variable. We report on two studies examining the role of age at entry to treatment and initial IQ in relationship to cognitive and adaptive outcomes in the Ontario province-wide program. Study 1 included 207 children aged 2–14 at entry. Age was modestly negatively correlated with several outcome variables; IQ was strongly predictive of most cognitive and adaptive outcomes. Age accounted for additional variance, beyond that accounted for by IQ, for cognitive outcomes, especially change in IQ. Children who made very large gains were all under age 6 at entry. Children who were over 8 years of age and/or had very low IQ showed uniformly poor outcomes. Study 2 was a comparison of IQ-matched younger (2–5 years) versus older (6–13 years) children (n = 60 each). The two groups of children, who were on the same initial trajectory, showed different outcomes. Only the younger group showed substantial cognitive gains. Results strongly argue for the importance of early intervention.

Introduction

A recent summary of meta-analyses of the efficacy of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI), also called Intensive Behavioral Intervention (IBI), for young children with autism states that, “the current evidence on the effectiveness of EIBI meets the threshold and criteria for the highest levels of evidence-based treatments… EIBI is the comprehensive treatment model for individuals with ASDs with the greatest amount of empirical support…” (Reichow, 2012, p. 518).

Most of the efficacy studies on IBI in the literature are small (10–30 children) and the children are often selected in some way, typically by age (usually younger than 4 years) and often by cognitive level (children with very low IQ and/or comorbid genetic/medical syndromes are excluded). The large gains documented have most often been on measures of global developmental level, with group data showing mean IQ gains of about 20 points. These major changes in developmental trajectory are considered the objective of IBI, so that children may potentially catch up to their peers and function normally (Lovaas, 1987). Gains in everyday adaptive skills have also been reported but have generally been smaller and less consistent, on the order of 10 points, with bigger gains in communication and social domains and minimal gains in daily living skills (e.g., Howard et al., 2005, Sallows and Graupner, 2005, Smith et al., 2000). However, it is important to note that outcomes in IBI are conspicuously variable in all studies. More research is needed to understand which children benefit, what treatment characteristics are important and, especially, how these apply in the context of the transfer to community effectiveness research rather than in the small model programs which have demonstrated IBI's efficacy (Reichow, 2012).

Two potentially important child characteristics which have received considerable attention in the literature are the children's chronological age and their cognitive level or IQ when they begin IBI. It can easily be argued a priori from several theoretical viewpoints why an early start might be important. A neurological perspective would suggest possible critical periods while there is still sufficient experience-dependent synaptic plasticity (Chugani, 2005). According to developmental theory, intervening early, before a child has fallen too far behind peers, would seem to be advantageous. Behavioral theory would also argue for developing adaptive repertoires of skills early to prevent, and not have to unlearn, repertoires which are less desirable. Furthermore, it makes intuitive sense to many parents and professionals that earlier is better. Perhaps surprisingly, several studies have reported that the age at which intervention started was not correlated with children's outcomes (e.g., Hayward et al., 2009, Lovaas, 1987, Sallows and Graupner, 2005). However, these studies only included very young children (2 years to 4 or 5 years in most studies). Eikeseth et al., 2002, Eikeseth et al., 2007 examined a group of children who were somewhat older (aged 4–7 years at intake) and also found age was uncorrelated with outcomes. It should be noted that restriction of range and small samples are likely to make correlation coefficients an insensitive measure in these studies. Another approach to examining the age question, which is to compare outcomes in younger versus older children within a sample, has been used in several studies. These studies typically do suggest that younger children have better outcomes than older children (Fenske et al., 1985, Harris and Handleman, 2000, Perry et al., 2008) and the same is true of Anderson, Avery, DiPietro, Edwards, and Christian (1987) based on the individual data in the paper. However, these studies all define good outcomes differently, and they use different definitions of older and younger. In summary, the evidence regarding early age being important is not very conclusive and it would be helpful to examine this question in a larger sample, which would provide more statistical power, and within a broader age range.

Initial IQ has long been known to be associated with the prognosis of individuals with autism (Howlin, 2005, Levy and Perry, 2011). In the IBI literature, IQ has frequently been reported to be highly correlated with outcomes (e.g., Harris and Handleman, 2000, Lovaas, 1987, Sallows and Graupner, 2005) although not unanimously (Cohen et al., 2006, Smith et al., 2000). However, it is important to recognize that this is likely the case regardless of treatment, as exemplified by results reported by Gabriels, Hill, Pierce, Rogers, and Wehner (2001) for generic treatment and the Eikeseth et al. (2002) eclectic comparison group. Thus, simple correlations of IQ at two time points are likely to be high regardless of what occurs between the two measurements. The importance of initial IQ as a predictor can only be tested by statistical procedures which control for the initial levels and/or examine magnitude of gains using IQ-point change or effect size metrics.

Several meta-analyses have now been conducted on the body of studies of IBI efficacy (see Reichow, 2012 for a summary and critical review). Of these various meta-analyses, three of them have examined predictors or moderators of outcome, including age and IQ (Makrygianni and Reed, 2010, Reichow and Wolery, 2009, Virues-Ortega, 2010). These meta-analyses included a different selection of studies and used different statistical methods but typically reported that treatment quantity (intensity, duration) and sometimes treatment model (supervisory model, inclusion of parents) had an impact on the magnitude of gains seen or effect size (ES), either pre–post difference and/or comparison of treatment to controls post-treatment. Initial age and IQ were examined in all three of these meta-analyses and were not found to be important moderators of treatment efficacy overall. However, it is important to remember the sample characteristics of the studies yielding these conclusions, as the mean IQ in most studies was in the 50s to 60s and the mean age was usually less than 4 years. If there are effects of age and IQ, they are more likely to be demonstrable in a more heterogeneous sample, where these effects could be examined individually and in combination.

The province of Ontario, Canada has had a publicly funded, province-wide IBI program since the year 2000. The program description, staffing model, and so on have been described in detail in earlier publications (e.g., Perry, 2002, Perry et al., 2008). The Ontario program has been the subject of a number of research studies, including several published in this journal. These studies of community effectiveness began with a retrospective, file-review study of pre–post changes in autism severity, cognitive, and adaptive functioning in 332 children who were aged 2–6 at program entry. This group of children was very heterogeneous on all developmental measures since there were no exclusion criteria for the program based on IQ or the presence of comorbid diagnoses. Children showed a wide range of outcomes (Perry et al., 2008).

Predictors of outcome in this sample were examined by Perry et al. (2011) and included age at entry, IQ, adaptive skill level, and autism severity. Looking at the role of age more specifically, Time 2 outcomes were moderately negatively correlated with age at entry. In addition, regressions to determine the relative strength of various predictors suggested that age accounted for a significant but modest amount of variance (from 1% to 6% of the variance depending on the measure). However, two additional sets of analyses suggested a non-linear relationship of initial age to children's outcomes. Children who started before the age of 4 years scored higher on all outcome measures than children who were 4 or older at intake and the children who had the best outcomes were significantly and substantially younger at entry (42 months versus 53 months) than all other outcome groups (Perry et al., 2011).

Flanagan, Perry, and Freeman (2012) then conducted a waitlist-controlled study of 142 children and found that predictors of outcome were different in the waitlist versus the IBI group. While initial developmental level (based on the Vineland as initial IQ was unavailable in many cases) was predictive of IQ outcome in both groups, age at entry to treatment was significant only for the IBI group, accounting for 13% of the variance in Time 2 IQ. This supports the notion that IBI is especially effective when started early.

Policy changes in 2005 resulted in the program becoming available to children of any age and children were no longer discharged at school age. Consequently, there were fewer discharges and waiting lists grew longer. This resulted in a sample of children beginning IBI over the age of 6. Considering that the previous literature has not demonstrated efficacy of IBI for children over age 6 or 7, Blacklock, Perry, and Dunn Geier (2013) examined this group. The results were, unfortunately, that this group of older children showed no significant gains in IQ or adaptive standard scores, although some children made clinically significant gains, mainly in adaptive behavior. Thus, both the magnitude and pattern of gains was different for this sample, relative to studies of younger children in the same program.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the value of age at entry and initial IQ as predictors of cognitive and adaptive outcomes in a large sample with a very wide range in both age and IQ, by combining the data from two previous Ontario studies (Blacklock et al., 2013, Perry et al., 2011). This combined dataset provides a unique opportunity to examine the role of age and IQ as predictors because of the large sample size (and therefore statistical power) and the extremely wide range of scores on both variables. This allows for a more sensitive examination of the importance of initial age and IQ in determining children's outcomes, individually and in combination, than has been possible before. Two sets of secondary analyses are reported. In Study 1 we examine the predictive power of IQ and age separately and combined in the large sample. In Study 2, we compared outcomes in a set of matched pairs of younger versus older children with equal IQ scores at intake.

Section snippets

Participants

The data for Study 1 included a combined sample from the two previously mentioned studies: the large Ontario study of 2- to 6-year-olds (Perry et al., 2008, Perry et al., 2011) and the study of children over 6 at entry into the IBI program (Blacklock et al., 2013). File data were obtained from all nine of the regional programs providing IBI throughout the province of Ontario. All children had a diagnosis of Autistic Disorder, PDD-NOS, or ASD, according to their files, based on various measures

Method

For the second study, we formed a matched sample of younger versus older children who were on similar developmental trajectories in order to explore the effect of initial age on outcomes independent of IQ. In order to do this, each child from the older sample (6–14 years) was individually yoke-matched, on the basis on initial IQ, with a child from the younger sample (2–5 years); resulting in 60 matched pairs (the remaining 8 children from the older sample could not be matched).

In order to

Discussion

This paper presents two secondary analyses of data from the province-wide IBI program in Ontario, Canada, in order to more fully understand the relative importance of initial age and initial IQ at entry to IBI in affecting children's outcomes, among a large and very heterogeneous sample. In Study 1, in the full combined sample (n = 207), we showed that initial IQ was strongly related to most outcomes, both cognitive and adaptive, consistent with much of the existing literature. However, even

Acknowledgements

Funding for this work was received from York University. We are grateful to all the regional IBI programs in Ontario for supporting our research. Thanks to Child Care Resources (Sudbury), Children's Hospital of Eastern Ontario (Ottawa), ErinoakKids (Mississauga), Hamilton Health Sciences (Hamilton), Hands: The Family Help Network (North Bay), Kinark Child and Family Services (Markham), Pathways for Children and Youth (Kingston), Surrey Place Center (Toronto), and Thames Valley Children's Centre

References (29)

  • D.C. Chugani

    Pharmacological intervention in autism: Targeting critical periods of brain development

    Clinical Neuropsychiatry

    (2005)
  • H. Cohen et al.

    Early intensive behavioral treatment: Replication of the UCLA model in a community setting

    Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics

    (2006)
  • S. Eikeseth et al.

    Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4- to 7-year-old children with autism: A 1-year comparison controlled study

    Behavior Modification

    (2002)
  • S. Eikeseth et al.

    Outcome for children with autism who began intensive behavioral treatment between ages 4 and 7: A comparison controlled study

    Behavior Modification

    (2007)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Parents’ experiences of the service pathway to an autism diagnosis for their child: What predicts an early diagnosis in Australia?

      2020, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      The early identification and diagnosis of autism is critical to ensure access to appropriate early intervention and support services. Intensive early intervention leads to improvements in language, cognition and adaptive functioning for many children on the autism spectrum (Dawson et al., 2010; Perry, Blacklock, & Dunn Geier, 2013; Rogers & Vismara, 2008), which may translate into substantial economic benefits for families and communities (Cidav et al., 2017). While support at any age is beneficial, younger children may benefit most from early intervention due to early brain plasticity (Dawson et al., 2012).

    • Interventions for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis of single-subject experimental studies

      2017, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Several participant, family and intervention characteristics can be expected to influence outcome of early intervention in young children with ASD, but knowledge on this topic is still mixed and limited. A number of child factors are sometimes mentioned as predictors of outcome, such as level of functioning, severity of ASD symptoms, and age at start of intervention (Kasari, Gulsrud, Freeman, Paparella, & Helleman, 2012; Perry, Blacklock, & Dunn Geier, 2013; Vivanti, Prior, Williams, & Dissanayake, 2014). Recent studies suggest that the earlier an intervention starts, the better the outcome (Guthrie et al., 2016; Orinstein et al., 2014; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2015b).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text