Elsevier

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Volume 6, Issue 3, Julyā€“September 2012, Pages 1194-1202
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Early language milestones predict later language, but not autism symptoms in higher functioning children with autism spectrum disorders

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.03.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Language ability is a known predictor of outcome in children with autism but plays a more controversial role for higher functioning children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). We studied the relationship between early language milestones and later structural language, adaptive functioning and autism symptoms in a sample of 76 children (mean ageĀ =Ā 9.1 years) with an ASD, using two different language milestones: first phrase by 24 or 36 months. After controlling for age and nonverbal abilities, retrospectively reported early language milestones were predictive of later structural language abilities, measured by a sentence repetition task, and adaptive communication skills, but not autism symptoms or adaptive social skills. Acquisition of phrase speech by 24 months was sensitive to language and communication impairments in our ASD group, the majority (84%) of which had already acquired phrase speech by 36 months of age. Early available and easily collectable milestone data may be a useful marker of later language performance even in higher functioning, verbal children on the autism spectrum. When a detailed assessment of language is not possible, data on early milestones may be useful for identifying children at-risk in clinical settings and for language phenotyping in the laboratory.

Highlights

ā–ŗ On-time language milestones predict communication outcomes at school age in HF-ASD. ā–ŗ On-time language milestones predict sentence repetition abilities at school age in HF-ASD. ā–ŗ Delayed language milestones identify an HF-ASD subgroup at risk for structural language impairment. ā–ŗ Early milestones data can inform treatment planning and language phenotyping in ASD.

Introduction

Language ability is an important predictor of outcome in children with autism (e.g., Howlin et al., 2004, Venter et al., 1992). More controversial is the role language plays for higher functioning children with autism spectrum disorders (HFASD). Language milestones and current language functioning are key elements of the diagnostic distinction between Asperger Disorder and autism (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), but investigators such as Loucas et al. (2008) have found that impaired structural language abilities are not predictive of the triad of social, communication and repetitive behavioral symptoms associated with autism. Although an essential starting point, the current nosology distinguishing autism from Asperger Disorder may have complicated our ability to determine the role of early language development in ASD because many investigators have relied on the DSM-IV-TR definitions of milestone delays (single words after 2 years and communicative phrases after 3 years), which are more lenient than commonly reported expectations for on time acquisition of speech (James, 1990, Sharp and Hillenbrand, 2008, Stott et al., 2002, Tager-Flusberg et al., 2009, Twatchtman-Cullen, 1998).

An alternative to dividing children on the autism spectrum by current DSM-IV TR diagnostic categories is to identify specific cognitive profiles or subtypes (Frith, 2001, Pennington, 2002) within ASD that may help parse the heterogeneity that can confound the search for genetic etiologies (Gottesman & Gould, 2003), and appropriately targeted interventions (Fisher and HappƩ, 2005, Riggs et al., 2006). For example, a common criticism of the distinction between high functioning autism and Asperger Disorder is that it does not differentiate children in a meaningful way beyond differences driven by intelligence (for reviews, see Matson et al., 2012, Witwer and Lecavalier, 2008; but see Ghazuiddin, 2008). In addition to intelligence, language functions, particularly structural language abilities, are promising candidates for subtyping children with ASD (Dawson et al., 2002, Howlin, 2005), because they are variable, and associated with both genetic differences and distinct intervention approaches (Tager-Flusberg, 2006). Furthermore, by investigating language abilities as a cognitive subtype (or language disability as a comorbidity) within ASD as opposed to embedding language in a diagnosis, we can investigate the specificity of language abilities throughout development, and test three competing hypotheses about early language abilities: they are not meaningful markers of outcome in autism; they are related to language outcome only; they are more generally related to ASD social and communication symptoms and adaptive outcomes.

Tager-Flusberg and Joseph (2003), among others, identify two language subtypes in verbal children with ASD: pragmatic deficits alone, and pragmatic deficits combined with impaired structural language (impaired phonology, vocabulary, syntax and morphology) and delays in the acquisition of language. Szatmari and colleagues (Bennett et al., 2008, Szatmari et al., 2009) found that what they termed ā€œstructural language impairmentā€ (StrLI) in children with ASD at 6ā€“8 years of age predicted adaptive and autistic behaviors at 17ā€“19 years of age. Loucas et al. (2008) report that StrLI in ASD did not affect presentation of autistic symptoms during late childhood, but it did relate to adaptive communication outcomes. Thus, several groups associate the presence of StrLI in individuals with ASD with long-term risk, but there is disagreement regarding whether the risk is specific to future language functioning or related to autism symptoms and outcomes more generally.

In addition to addressing the specificity of early language development for predicting later functioning in ASD, a second unanswered question involves what constitutes an effective early marker of StrLI in ASD. Are language milestones, one element associated with StrLI, in and of themselves a predictive marker in ASD? Mayes, Calhoun, and Crites (2001) found that in ASD, acquisition of single words by 24 months and phrase speech by 36 months did not predict autism symptoms or current language abilities. On the other hand, Szatmari, Archer, Fisman, Streiner, and Wilson (1995) found that language milestones were predictive of a range of autism symptoms in children less than six years of age and social reciprocity in older children (mean ageĀ =Ā 11 years). Their milestone criteria were spontaneous phrase speech by 36 months and an absence of marked or persistent delayed echolalia, pronoun reversal and neologisms. In a comprehensive longitudinal follow-up study, Bennett et al. (2008) used the same criteria to predict outcome through 15ā€“17 years of age. They report that structural language skills assessed at 6ā€“8 years of age were more predictive of adaptive outcomes and autism symptoms in later adolescence than milestones or language skills at 4ā€“6 years of age. Milestones were, however, at least as strongly correlated with daily living and social adaptive skills as was the presence of StrLI at age 6ā€“8 in two of their three follow-up assessments. While far from conclusive, these investigations indicate the possibility that milestones are related to later symptoms and adaptive outcome.

Further investigation of the relationship between early language milestones and outcome is warranted for many reasons. There is some evidence that easily attained milestone data has predictive power regarding ASD outcomes into adolescence, but there is disagreement regarding exactly what aspects of outcome it predicts. It is also unclear what age at milestone attainment would be most sensitive to later outcome in children with HFASD. Some groups have relied on the DSM-IV-TR milestone criteria of 36 months while 24 months is more commonly used milestone for phrase speech in the study of typical development (James, 1990, Sharp and Hillenbrand, 2008, Stott et al., 2002, Twatchtman-Cullen, 1998), and an NIH working group on language development in ASD (Tager-Flusberg et al., 2009) recommends a range of 18ā€“30 months as the time period in which word combinations are expected. Another reason to investigate milestones is their potential to inform developmental trajectories. As argued by Volkmar, State, and Klin (2009), attempts to identify cognitive subtypes in ASD must address the developmental trajectories of those abilities, for which milestones can be a useful tool. If early language milestones are shown to meaningfully predict later outcomes, they can inform research and treatment. They might offer an easy, early-available method for parsing the heterogeneity in ASD that confounds many inquiries into its biology. For example, language milestones have previously been used as phenotypic markers in autism genetics studies (e.g., Alarcon, Yonan, Gilliam, Cantor, & Geschwind, 2005). Additionally, pediatricians and other practitioners can use milestone information when they are predicting trajectories and as an impetus for early language evaluation and intervention, even for children without intellectual disability.

In this investigation, we query whether the age of attainment of early language milestones in children with ASD predicts better outcome at school age. In an effort to explore the relationship between age of language milestone attainment and later development in higher functioning children with ASD, we compare the predictive value of relatively stringent language milestone attainment to the more lenient milestones described in DSM-IV-TR. Specifically, we investigate parent retrospective reports of early milestones in relationship to later autism symptoms, adaptive functioning, and structural language. Szatmari, Bryson, Boyle, Streiner, and Duku (2003) and Klin et al. (2007) have highlighted that outcome, or the ability to function effectively in mainstream settings, is not solely driven by severity of autism symptoms, but also by adaptive behaviors. Therefore we report on the relationship of early milestones to Vineland Adaptive Behavior scores as well as Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule scores (ADOS; Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999). A sentence repetition task was used as a marker of structural language for two reasons: it captures core grammar, syntax, and morphology, which appear to be the greatest areas of impairment in the ASD with StrLI subtype (Loucas et al., 2008, Tager-Flusberg and Cooper, 1999); and it is described as the best overall marker of communication impairment both in typically developing children and in children with autism (Botting and Conti-Ramsden, 2003, Conti-Ramsden et al., 2001).

In this study, we investigate whether age of language milestone attainment is: (A) not related to outcome at school age; (B) narrowly related to later structural language/communication abilities; or (C) more broadly related to adaptive behavior and autism symptoms in children with HFASD. A second aim of this study is to examine the sensitivity of meeting two different language milestone criteria: first word and phrase speech milestones by 15 and 24 months, or by 24 and 36 months, respectively, in predicting outcomes at school age.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants consisted of a clinically referred sample of 76 children with a full scale, verbal or nonverbal estimate of IQĀ ā‰„Ā 70 and an ASD diagnosis (autism nĀ =Ā 34, Asperger Disorder nĀ =Ā 24; PDD-NOS nĀ =Ā 18). All participants received a comprehensive multidisciplinary team evaluation in the Washington, DC metropolitan area. Participants with evidence of a co-morbid neurological or a known genetic disorder were excluded. Data were collected as part of a clinical evaluation, thus not all participants

Descriptive statistics

As described in Table 2, the correlation between onset of first words and phrase speech was high (rĀ =Ā .82) in our sample suggesting that these two variables reflect the same underlying construct. Moreover, except as noted below, there were essentially no differences in the relationship between first words and phrase speech milestones and the outcome variables. As a result, we focus our description of results on presentation of the 24- and 36-month phrase speech milestones.

As shown in Table 1,

Discussion

In this investigation we found that age of early language milestones is predictive of specific language abilities and adaptive communication skills in a group of school-aged children with ASD. While language acquisition milestones are useful in this sample for capturing later language performance even in higher functioning children with ASD, they do not predict other adaptive skills or autism social and communication symptoms. Our data support the view that early milestones represent an

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, this investigation has implications for future research and clinical practice. In settings in which a detailed assessment of language is not possible, data on early milestones provides important information even in verbal, HFASD. Milestones may be a useful marker of early language development in longitudinal studies, more of which are needed (Volkmar et al., 2009). Whether in the pediatrician's office, or in large-scale research projects, the phrases by 24 months

Conflict of interest

None to declare.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank: Jennifer L. Sokoloff and Richard B. Roberson III for data entry; Ben Yerys for helpful comments; and the people with autism and their families who have educated us over the years. LK, GLW, AM, and DOB were supported by the NIH, National Institute of Mental Health Intramural Research Program. LK was also supported by an award from The Singer Family Foundation and The Isadore and Bertha Gudelsky Family Foundation.

References (56)

  • G. Dawson et al.

    Defining the broader phenotype of autism: Genetic, brain and behavioral perspectives

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2002)
  • K. Dworzynski et al.

    Relationship between symptom domains in autism spectrum disorders: A population based twin study

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2009)
  • N. Fisher et al.

    A training study of theory of mind and executive function in children with autistic spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2005)
  • M. Ghazuiddin

    Defining the behavioral phenotype of Asperger syndrome

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2008)
  • W.A. Goldberg et al.

    Use of home videotapes to confirm parental reports of regression in autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

    (2008)
  • K. Gotham et al.

    Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2009)
  • I.I. Gottesman et al.

    The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • A.B. Hollingshead

    Four factor index of social status

    (1975)
  • P. Howlin

    Outcomes in autism spectrum disorders

  • P. Howlin et al.

    Adult outcome for children with autism

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2004)
  • S.L. James

    Normal language acquisition

    (1990)
  • A. Klin et al.

    Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2007)
  • S.L. Korkman et al.

    NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment

    (1998)
  • J.E. Lainhart et al.

    Head circumference and height in autism: A study by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism

    American Journal of Medical Genetics

    (2006)
  • A. Le Couteur et al.

    Autism diagnostic interview: A standardized investigator-based instrument

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1989)
  • C. Lord et al.

    Quantifying the phenotype in autism spectrum disorders

    Neuropsychiatric Genetics

    (2001)
  • C. Lord et al.

    Autism diagnostic observation schedule-WPS edition

    (1999)
  • C. Lord et al.

    Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1994)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Detection without further processing or processing without automatic detection? Differential ERP responses to lexical-semantic processing in toddlers at high clinical risk for autism and language disorder

      2021, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Even if language delays are not included in the DSM-5 as a diagnostic criterium, deficits in language skills in the first years of life are a hallmark feature of ASD. According to reports, children with ASD do not only utter their first words (Charman et al., 2003; Matson, Mahan, Kozlowski, & Shoemaker, 2010) and utterances (Grandgeorge et al., 2009; Kenworthy et al., 2012) later but also show smaller receptive vocabularies, more superļ¬cial deļ¬nitions of words, and poorer understanding of relatedness between words compared to their typically developing peers (Boucher, 2011; Haebig, Kaushanskaya, & Ellis Weismer, 2015; Henderson, Powell, Gaskell, & Norbury, 2014; McGregor & Bean, 2012). Although many typical symptoms can differentiate children with early signs of ASD from late talkers (i.e., lower degree of spontaneous pretend play, expression of joint attentional intentions and use of conventional gestures and greater extent of repetitive movements and unusual vocalization, Paul, Chawarska, & Volkmar, 2008), the two populations show striking similarity if only the observed language impairment is considered.

    • Language predictors of autism spectrum disorder in young children with tuberous sclerosis complex

      2020, Epilepsy and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, early language screening could serve as a less costly and less time-intensive approach to identifying infants with TSC at risk for ASD, who then could be targeted for more definitive evaluation and/or therapeutic intervention. It should be noted, however, that our findings do contradict an earlier study by Kenworthy et al., who found that core language abilities had no impact on autism diagnosis but were rather representative of potential comorbid conditions that alter prognosis rather than diagnosis [26]. That study excluded patients with ASD with genetic or molecular etiologies, including TSC, so direct comparison of results is cautioned.

    • Hearing me hearing you: Reciprocal effects between child and parent language in autism and typical development

      2019, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Studies of child-based variability in TD children have been limited, with twin studies being the major exceptions (Colledge et al., 2002; Reznick, Corley, Robinson, & Matheny, 1997). On the other hand, research on the variability of language development in children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) has emphasized child-based factors such as verbal and nonverbal intelligence and autism severity, which account for significant variance in these childrenā€™s language outcomes (Anderson et al., 2007; Bang & Nadig, 2015; Bopp, Mirenda, & Zumbo, 2009; Ellis Weismer & Kover, 2015; Howlin, Savage, Moss, Tempier, & Rutter, 2014; Kenworthy et al., 2012; Magiati, Tay, & Howlin, 2014; Munson et al., 2008; Paul, Chawarska, Cicchetti, & Volkmar, 2008; Szatmari et al., 2009; Venker et al., 2015; Wodka, Mathy, & Kalb, 2013), while investigations of the structure and content of parental input are only just beginning (Bang & Nadig, 2015; Nadig & Bang, 2017; Venker et al., 2015). In the current paper, we combine these approaches in a longitudinal comparison of the language trajectories of initially language-matched TD children and children with ASD.

    • Language Development in Autism

      2015, Neurobiology of Language
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text