Elsevier

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Volume 2, Issue 3, July–September 2008, Pages 430-446
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Effects of iconicity on requesting with the Picture Exchange Communication System in children with autism spectrum disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2007.09.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Research on graphic symbol learning suggests that symbols with a greater visual resemblance to their referents (greater iconicity) are more easily learned. The iconicity hypothesis has not yet been explored within the intervention protocol of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS). Within the PECS protocol, participants do not point to a symbol but exchange the symbol for an object. The purpose of this study was to examine whether children learn to request more readily with PECS when the symbols involved are highly iconic versus symbols that are low in iconicity. An adapted alternating treatments design combined with a multiple baseline design across subjects was used to evaluate the effectiveness and efficiency of symbol learning under two conditions: high iconicity and low iconicity. Four students with autism or pervasive developmental disorders between the ages of six and nine years participated. Results indicated that students learned to request desired objects under both conditions, lending further support for the effectiveness of PECS. There was little to no difference, however, in the effectiveness and efficiency of requesting between the two conditions during Phases I and II of PECS training. Thus learners do not benefit from symbols that bear more resemblance with their referents during the first two phases of PECS instruction.

Section snippets

Participants

Four children between the ages of 6 and 10 with a diagnosis of autism or Pervasive Developmental Disorder (PDD) participated. A licensed psychologist, pediatrician, or neurologist diagnosed the children according to the criteria set forth by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual: Fourth Edition (DSM-IV). Additionally, participants had to meet the following criteria: (a) have little to no functional speech (no more than 10 spoken words), (b) rely primarily on pre-linguistic means of

Results

Data were plotted graphically to examine the effectiveness and efficiency of symbol learning during three phases of PECS using Blissymbols and PCS. Daily probe results are presented in Fig. 1, and intervention results are reported in Fig. 2 (missing data points are a result of school absences). Specifically, the percentage of correct requests per session and the number of sessions to criterion for each phase of PECS instruction were recorded. All students achieved mastery in Phases I and II of

Discussion

This study adds to the current body of literature documenting the effectiveness of PECS instruction in teaching students with autism/PDD to request using graphic symbols (e.g., Ganz & Simpson, 2004; Tincani, 2004; Yoder and Stone, 2006a, Yoder and Stone, 2006b). The results of this study also showed that participants mastered Phases I and II of PECS with very little difference between the two experimental conditions: PCS and Blissymbols. Three students did not achieve mastery of Phase III

References (31)

  • R.K. Koul et al.

    Comparison of graphic symbol learning in individuals with aphasia and right hemisphere brain damage

    Brain and Language

    (1998)
  • J. Sigafoos et al.

    Assessing correspondence following acquisition of an exchanged-based communication system

    Research in Developmental Disabilities

    (2007)
  • American Psychological Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (1994)
  • D.R. Beukelman et al.

    Augmentative and alternative communication: Supporting children and adults with complex communication needs

    (2005)
  • K. Bloomberg et al.

    The comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets

    Journal of Speech and Hearing Research

    (1990)
  • A.S. Bondy et al.

    The picture exchange communication system

    Behavior Modification

    (2001)
  • M.H. Charlop-Christy et al.

    Using the picture exchange communication system (PECS) with children with autism: Assessment of PECS acquisition, speech, social-communicative behavior, and problem behavior

    Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis

    (2002)
  • L.A. Frost et al.

    The Picture Exchange Communication System training manual

    (1994)
  • D.R. Fuller

    Initial study into the effects of translucency and complexity on the learning of Blissymbols by children and adults with normal cognitive abilities

    Augmentative and Alternative Communication

    (1997)
  • J.B. Ganz et al.

    Effects on communicative requesting and speech development of the picture exchange communication system in children with characteristics of autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2004)
  • B.I. Hurlbut et al.

    Nonvocal language acquisition in adolescents with severe physical disabilities: Blissymbols versus iconic stimulus formats

    Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis

    (1982)
  • R.K. Koul et al.

    Effects of symbol, referent, and instructional variables on the acquisition of aided and unaided symbols by individuals with autism spectrum disorders

    Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities

    (2001)
  • E. Kozleski

    Visual symbol acquisition by students with autism

    Exceptionality

    (1991)
  • L.L. Lloyd et al.

    The role of iconicity in augmentative and alternative communication symbol learning

  • L.L. Lloyd et al.

    Augmentative and alternative communication: A handbook of principles and practices

    (1997)
  • Cited by (36)

    • Comparisons of intervention components within augmentative and alternative communication systems for individuals with developmental disabilities: A review of the literature

      2013, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      One study measuring maintenance also did not support an effect for symbol iconicity (Angermeier et al., 2008). Phase III data (i.e. requests involving discriminations between symbols) for both acquisition and maintenance were, however, inconclusive for a majority of participants in the Angermeier et al. (2008) study. In contrast, Koul and Schlosser (2004) provided suggestive evidence (limited by the number of IOA sessions and lack of treatment fidelity) that high iconicity symbols on an SGD were more likely to be receptively identified than low iconicity symbols.

    • Comparative efficacy of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) versus a speech-generating device: Effects on requesting skills

      2013, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Some literature suggests the degree of iconicity affects the learnability of the graphic symbol (Fuller, 1997; Goossens, 1983; Koul, Schlosser, & Sancibrian, 2001; Nail-Chiwetalu, 1991). Yet, Angermeier et al. (2008) found no differences when graphic symbol iconicity was assessed during PECS. Because it is not clear how much symbol iconicity affects PECS learning and given that participants in this study had difficulty discriminating between several picture symbols, it is possible that picture iconicity affected learning and should be investigated further.

    • Meta-analysis of PECS with individuals with ASD: Investigation of targeted versus non-targeted outcomes, participant characteristics, and implementation phase

      2012, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      PECS consists of six phases of progressively complex training, starting with exchanging a picture with a communicative partner in order to obtain a desired item (Frost & Bondy, 2002). In many cases, especially in individuals with severe disabilities, only the first three phases of PECS have been taught (e.g., Angermeier, Schlosser, Luiselli, Harrington, & Carter, 2008; Carre, Le Grice, Blampied, & Walker, 2009; Bondy & Frost, 1994). Teaching through phrase three teaches picture exchange and icon discrimination but does not include teaching phrases (e.g., I SEE, I WANT), adjectives, or discrimination by color, shape, or other features (see Frost & Bondy, 2002; Ganz, Simpson, et al., in press).

    • An aggregate study of single-case research involving aided AAC: Participant characteristics of individuals with autism spectrum disorders

      2011, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Frea, Arnold, & Vittimberga (2001) implemented a picture exchange intervention that provided opportunities for a non-verbal 4 year old boy with autism to engage in choice-making to obtain preferred items, resulting in a reduction of aggressive behaviors. A variety of AAC interventions have also been implemented with elementary children (e.g., Angermeier, Schlosser, Luiselli, Harrington, & Carter, 2008; Buckley & Newchok, 2005; Schlosser, Blischak, Belfiore, Bartley, & Barnett, 1998). Tincani, Crozier, & Alazetta (2006) compared the use of sign language and PECS to increase the use of requesting behavior for two children with autism, ages 5 and 6.

    • Iconicity and ease of learning of the ARASAAC pictographic symbols

      2018, Revista de Logopedia, Foniatria y Audiologia
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text