Elsevier

Psychoneuroendocrinology

Volume 87, January 2018, Pages 1-8
Psychoneuroendocrinology

Classroom social experiences in early elementary school relate to diurnal cortisol levels

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.09.025Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The association of peer acceptance and peer non-acceptance on changes in diurnal cortisol concentration in elementary school children was examined.

  • Low peer acceptance was associated with heightened diurnal cortisol output and lower cortisol reductions across the day.

  • Peer non-acceptance and the interaction with peer acceptance (peer rejection) were not associated with diurnal cortisol.

  • Findings emphasize the association between poor appraisal among classroom peers and children’s heightened HPA-axis activation.

Abstract

Social stress has been linked to altered hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocorticol (HPA) axis activation. During elementary school, children can become exposed to negative peer relations, such as poor appraisal among classroom peers, which is considered a social stressor. However, little is known about the association between classroom peer appraisal and the physiological stress system in children. The goal of this study was to examine the association of peer acceptance and peer non-acceptance with diurnal cortisol concentrations in 222 children from 20 mainstream elementary schools (Mage = 6.97 years, SD = 0.99, 55% boys) in the Netherlands. Saliva samples were collected at awakening, 30 min post-awakening, at noon and at 8 pm during a weekend day. From these assessments, the Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR), diurnal cortisol concentration (AUCg) and diurnal cortisol slope were calculated. Peer nominations of peer acceptance (being liked), and peer non-acceptance (being disliked) were collected across a one year interval. Associations were controlled for peer victimization, age, sex and SES and children’s levels of emotional problems and behavioural problems. Results showed that low peer acceptance was associated with heightened diurnal cortisol concentration (i.e., heightened AUGg), lower cortisol reductions across the day (i.e., less decreasing cortisol slope) and heightened cortisol awakening response (i.e., heightened CAR). Peer non-acceptance and the interaction between peer acceptance and peer non-acceptance (known as peer rejection) were not associated with AUCg, cortisol slope or the CAR. The findings emphasize the association between poor appraisal among classroom peers and children’s heightened HPA-axis activation. This underscores the importance of the physiological stress system in studying the consequences of negative peer relations in children.

Introduction

With the transition to formal schooling, children are exposed to potential social stressors such as negative peer appraisal (Rubin et al., 2006). Exposure to social stress may have long-term consequences on hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning (Cohen et al., 2007). Social stress, such as constituted by negative experiences in the peer context (peer victimization) are possibly associated with upregulated HPA-axis functioning in elementary school children (Peters et al., 2011). Approximately 10–15% of all children become poorly accepted or rejected by their peers (Woodward and Fergusson, 2000). Yet, little is known about the association of this type of social stress in early elementary school and the physiological stress system in children. The goal of this study is to assess the unique and combined association between peer acceptance and peer non-acceptance with HPA-axis functioning in early elementary school children in the Netherlands. Dysregulation of the HPA-axis functioning has previously been linked to the onset of problematic behaviour and emotional problems during childhood (Gunnar, 2000, Shirtcliff and Essex, 2008; El-Sheikh et al., 2008; Saridjan et al., 2014). Thus, if research results reveal a biological embeddedness of social stress in children, this would indicate that social environmental factors associated with altered HPA-axis activation in children need to be targeted for early prevention of mental health problems (Adam et al., 2007).

Both higher and lower than average diurnal levels of cortisol have been suggested to be maladaptive. Elevated and lowered diurnal cortisol levels have been linked to memory, attention problems and failures in coping capacities (Flynn and Rudolph, 2007). Cortisol concentrations typically rise directly following awakening and decline throughout the day, with a slight peak around noon (Cohen et al., 2007). The cortisol awakening curve (CAR) is typically assessed by the change in cortisol concentrations from directly following awakening to 30 min after awakening. Two other cortisol measures represent daytime changes in cortisol concentration, namely the daytime cortisol curve and the cortisol slope. The daytime cortisol curve (Area Under the Curve with respect to the Ground; the AUCg) represents an estimation of the total cortisol output during the day. The cortisol slope represents the diurnal change of cortisol concentration (Saxbe, 2008).

The CAR represents the preparation of the body to face daily hassles and is therefore linked to overall diurnal cortisol output (Clow et al., 2010; Adam et al., 2007). A larger than usual CAR may signal anticipation to stressful situations and has been associated with frequent arguments at home in children (Michels et al., 2012). Whether associations between (peer) stressors and CAR exist in childhood is unknown as previous studies in childhood samples (e.g., see Gunnar et al., 2003) did not study CAR but focused on cortisol levels instead. However, a larger CAR than average may be expected in children experiencing frequent peer stressors.

Several studies have linked individual differences in daily cortisol output to (chronic) stress exposure. For instance, negative social experiences, such as frequent arguments between parents at home, were related to a more negative cortisol slope in six-year-old children (DeCaro et al., 2008). Cortisol production might be downregulated after facing extended stress exposure for several months, as the HPA-axis becomes overstrained. Through the negative feedback circuit this results in suppressed output of CRH and ACTH by acting on glucocorticoid receptors in the brain, ultimately leading to cortisol output rebounds below normal (Miller et al., 2007). Indeed, lowered diurnal cortisol levels were observed in 3/4-year-old children after facing adverse early child care for two years (Koss et al., 2014). Additionally, active classroom exclusion by peers was related to downregulation of the cortisol diurnal curve and heightened cortisol levels at school in 8–10 years old children (Peters et al., 2011). Peer rejection was associated with heightened cortisol levels in preschool children (Gunnar et al., 2003). However, in this latter study cortisol levels were only assessed once during the day, thereby impeding studying whether peer rejection was linked to the CAR, cortisol slope and the AUCg. It needs to be investigated whether the cortisol slope, AUCg and CAR are similarly influenced by peer appraisal as these indicators of HPA axis functioning have a different connotation (Saxbe, 2008). Thus, although negative peer experiences may represent (prolonged) stressful social experiences (MacDonald and Leary, 2005;, 2001;, 2007), our understanding of the association between peer relations and the cortisol awakening response as well as diurnal cortisol output in early elementary school children is far from complete.

In this study, we associate individual differences in diurnal cortisol concentration in elementary school children with early elementary school classroom peer acceptance and peer non-acceptance. Peer acceptance is typically assessed by asking classroom peers to nominate who is liked in the classroom (Coie et al., 1982). In contrast, peer non-acceptance indicates who is disliked by their classroom peers (Coie et al., 1982). Scoring low on peer acceptance does not necessarily mean that a child is actively disliked. The child may be mostly neglected in the social evaluation (Coie et al., 1990). However, peer-acceptance and peer non-acceptance may also jointly excerpt their influence. That is, low scores on peer acceptance, combined with high scores on peer non-acceptance is referred to as peer rejection (Deater-Deckard, 2001, Coie et al., 1990). Peer rejection has been previously described as especially troublesome for children (Williams, 2001, Williams, 2007). Although poor peer appraisal is described as stressful for children, it is important to note that asking children how they like or dislike peers does not necessarily mean that they actively show negative behaviours toward the child. Peer victimization is considered a behavioural manifestation of peer rejection, as it involves actively trying to harm the victim and their social relations with peers (Olweus, 1986, 1993). Previous research suggested that there exists an association between peer victimization and heightened cortisol levels in children (Peters et al., 2011). However, it is still unclear whether mere peer appraisal differences are associated with altered cortisol levels in children. This is important as mere peer appraisal might not always be visible in classroom behavior but might already be associated with altered daily cortisol levels in children as proposed by Gunnar et al. (2003).

In the present study, we aimed to explore the unique and possible joint influence of peer acceptance and non-acceptance with diurnal cortisol output in 222 early elementary school children. We expected that especially peer rejection, that is, the combination of low peer acceptance and high peer non-acceptance, is associated with heightened individual differences in daily cortisol levels (Gunnar et al., 2003). Specifically, we expected peer rejection to be linked to heightened CAR, with lower reductions in cortisol across the day, and with higher cortisol levels across the day. In all models, behavioural and emotional problems, as well as peer relational victimization, will be controlled for as such problems in itself have been repeatedly linked to altered HPA axis activity (Bruce et al., 2002, Peters et al., 2011; Rudolph et al., 2010; Saridjan et al., 2014) and coincide with the peer social stressors rejection and poor acceptance (Blackhart et al., 2007, van Lier and Koot, 2010).

Section snippets

Participants

Data of this study are part of the Happy Children, Happy Adolescents? (HCHA) project, a longitudinal study among 1624 children on their socio-cognitive and behavioural development in the school context (M age = 6.0 years, SD = 0.46 at the start of the study; 50% boys). Children were first assessed in fall 2011. The participants lived in the east and center of the Netherlands, had mostly a Dutch-Caucasian background (90%), and the family socioeconomic status (SES) was low in 10% of the sample.

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics for the study variables, and the correlations between the study variables are depicted in Table 1. Cortisol concentrations of the four measurement points are presented in Table 2. Results in Table 1 showed significant negative correlations were found between peer acceptance and AUCg (r = −0.17, p < 0.05), between peer acceptance and the diurnal cortisol slope (r = −0.17, p < 0.05), and between peer non-acceptance and peer acceptance (r = −0.35, p < 0.01). Relational

Discussion

This study examined the association between peer appraisal and diurnal cortisol levels in a sample of children in the early years of mainstream elementary schools in the Netherlands. Our results revealed that low peer acceptance (scoring low on being liked) was associated with heightened CAR, heightened AUCg and attenuated decreases in cortisol across the day, as indexed by the diurnal cortisol slope. Non-acceptance by peers (being disliked) or peer rejection (scoring low on being liked and

Funding

This work was funded by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, program medium sized investments, nr: 480-13-006 and the ZonMW subsidy: Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development, program Youth, nr: 15700.4001. This study was supported by the European Research Council, under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme, Grants 646594 and 648082.

Conflict of interest

None.

Authorship

Pia Behnsen: (1) the conception and design of the study, analysis and interpretation of

References (57)

  • M.J. Meaney et al.

    Epigenetic mechanisms of perinatal programming of hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal function and health

    Trends Mol. Med.

    (2007)
  • N. Michels et al.

    Negative life events, emotions and psychological difficulties as determinants of salivary cortisol in Belgian primary school children

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • J.C. Pruessner et al.

    Free cortisol levels after awakening: a reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity

    Life Sci.

    (1997)
  • N.S. Saridjan et al.

    The longitudinal association of the diurnal cortisol rhythm with internalizing and externalizing problems in pre-schoolers. The Generation R Study

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • T. Stalder et al.

    Associations between the cortisol awakening response and heart rate variability

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2011)
  • S.E. Watamura et al.

    Cortisol patterns at home and child care: afternoon differences and evening recovery in children attending very high quality full-day center-based child care

    J. App. Dev. Psychologysychol.

    (2009)
  • J.M. Wolf et al.

    Chronic stress, salivary cortisol, and α-amylase in children with asthma and healthy children

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2008)
  • E.K. Adam et al.

    Social regulation of the adrenocortical response to stress in infants, children, and adolescents

    Hum. Behav. Learn. Dev. Brain: Atyp. Dev.

    (2007)
  • M. Berset et al.

    Work characteristics as predictors of physiological recovery on weekends

    Scand. J. Work Environ. Health

    (2009)
  • B.B. Brown

    Peer Groups and Peer Cultures

    (1990)
  • S. Cohen et al.

    Psychological stress and disease

    JAMA

    (2007)
  • J.D. Coie et al.

    Dimensions and types of social status: a cross-age perspective

    Dev. Psychol.

    (1982)
  • J.D. Coie et al.

    Peer group behaviour and social status

    Peer Rejection in Childhood

    (1990)
  • N.R. Crick et al.

    Children’s treatment by peers: victims of relational and overt aggression

    Dev. Psychopath.

    (1996)
  • J.A. DeCaro

    Methodological considerations in the use of salivary α‐amylase as a stress marker in field research

    Am. J. Hum. Biol

    (2008)
  • K. Deater-Deckard

    Annotation: recent research examining the role of peer relationships in the development of psychopathology

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • M. El-Sheikh et al.

    Cortisol and children’s adjustment: The moderating role of sympathetic nervous system activity

    J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.

    (2008)
  • M. Flynn et al.

    Perceptual asymmetry and youths’ responses to stress: understanding vulnerability to depression

    Cogn. Emot.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (18)

    • Peers and psychopathology

      2023, Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Health, First Edition
    • Unique and interactive associations of proactive and reactive aggression with cortisol secretion

      2022, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, as far as REA is concerned, some evidence suggests it could be related to a flatter cortisol slope. Indeed, a flatter cortisol slope has been identified as the most consistent correlate of characteristics commonly associated with REA (but not PROA), including fearfulness and poorer relationship quality (Behnsen et al., 2018; Van Hulle et al., 2017). Furthermore, later day cortisol levels have been described as a potential biological index of cumulative family stress (Van Hulle et al., 2012), with a flatter diurnal cortisol slope denoting poorer family functioning (Zalewski et al., 2012).

    • Heightened neural sensitivity to social exclusion in boys with a history of low peer preference during primary school

      2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Neural responses to social exclusion of 55 primary school boys (mean age = 10.40 years, SD = 0.74, median = 10.54, range = 8.32–11.66 years) were collected in June 2016 – May 2017 during a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session. The participants were selected from a longitudinal classroom-based study on the social, emotional and cognitive development of children during primary school (Behnsen et al., 2018; de Wilde et al., 2016; Tieskens et al., 2018). Only boys were selected for the following reasons.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text