Elsevier

Psychoneuroendocrinology

Volume 36, Issue 8, September 2011, Pages 1127-1136
Psychoneuroendocrinology

Assessing stress reactivity indexed via salivary cortisol in preschool-aged children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.02.003Get rights and content

Summary

Identifying a stressor paradigm that elicits mean increases in salivary cortisol in young children has proven elusive, possibly due to characteristics of the paradigms used and how and when cortisol is sampled. We therefore examined the validity of a standardized task (adapted from Lewis and Ramsay, 2002) and procedures developed to assess cortisol reactivity in 215 preschool-aged children. Children participated in a standardized stress task during a home visit, which was videorecorded for future coding. Salivary cortisol samples were obtained at baseline and 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50 min post-stress. In support of the validity of the task, significant increases in cortisol levels from baseline were found, followed by a significant decline, and a quadratic function provided a good fit to the data. Children also showed a significant increase in negative emotions and a decrease in positive emotions over the course of the stress task. Results indicate that the task successfully elicited the hypothesized cortisol response in 3-year-old children.

Introduction

Individual differences in stress sensitivity have been strongly implicated in the etiology of major depression and anxiety disorders (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001, McFarlane et al., 2005), suggesting that early-emerging indicators of heightened stress sensitivity could be used to develop targeted preventative and early intervention strategies. As befitting a topic with such important implications, numerous studies have aimed to measure childhood stress reactivity, operationalizing sensitivity to stress via activity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis in response to standardized stress paradigms. Cortisol, the hormonal end-product of the HPA system, can be readily indexed noninvasively through salivary assays and is a potentially useful tool in developmental psychopathology research aimed at characterizing stress responsivity in childhood. However, the methods used to elicit cortisol reactivity in children vary widely, and the resulting literature is, accordingly, mixed and inconclusive.

Gunnar et al. (2009) recently provided an excellent review of this literature, highlighting the substantial variability in methodologies used to elicit cortisol in early childhood. In this review, the authors note that only a small minority of studies of young children (i.e., ages 2–5 years old) report a mean increase in child cortisol in response to standardized stressor paradigms (Gunnar et al., 2009). Gunnar and colleagues concluded that without “concerted effort to develop reliable stress tasks for this age range, we may be left with a broad span of development during which we cannot really assess reactivity of the axis in typically developing children” (p. 964).

Despite the absence of a consistent literature to draw upon, Gunnar et al. (2009) made recommendations regarding task characteristics that may elicit a cortisol increase in young children. For example, Gunnar et al. observed that leading children to believe that they have failed on a task that younger children are capable of doing may elicit a cortisol increase. This suggestion is remarkably consistent with results from a recent meta-analysis of cortisol reactivity in adults (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004), which showed that tasks that entailed a socially evaluative component, along with perceived uncontrollability and motivated performance, produced the greatest and most prolonged cortisol response. While this meta-analysis included studies of adults only, designing developmentally appropriate downward extensions of tasks that elicit cortisol responses in older samples would represent a valuable contribution to the field, as research aimed at examining stability and change in cortisol reactivity across development will be facilitated by tasks that are similar in terms of the nature of the stress manipulation.

An array of methodological considerations may have influenced the extent to which previous research has been able to accurately characterize early-emerging cortisol reactivity in young children. The vast majority of studies of cortisol reactivity to laboratory stressors have assessed cortisol at two time points only (Earle et al., 1999, Matthews et al., 2001, Roy et al., 2001, Smeekens et al., 2007; for exceptions, see Mills et al., 2008, Zoccola et al., 2008). This practice may have limited the extent to which studies can accurately capture peaks in cortisol response, as individuals, including children, vary in terms of how rapidly a maximum cortisol response is expressed post-stressor (Goldberg et al., 2003, Gunnar and Talge, 2008, Lewis and Ramsay, 2002, Lopez-Duran et al., 2009). Additionally, obtaining a minimal number of samples hinders the ability to characterize post-stress downregulation/recovery, which may also have implications for psychopathology risk.

Other methodological considerations include where and when assessments of cortisol reactivity are conducted. Many previous studies have assessed cortisol reactivity in children in laboratory settings (Lewis and Ramsay, 2002, Lopez-Duran et al., 2009, Smeekens et al., 2007). While laboratory settings have advantages, this approach raises the concern that the novelty of the setting itself might influence children's cortisol levels. Research by Tottenham et al. (2001) indicates that this is so, by showing that assays of children yield higher levels of baseline cortisol when samples are collected in the laboratory versus the home (as cited in Gunnar and Talge, 2008, p. 351). This indicates that the baseline cortisol levels reported in many studies reflect a psychophysiological reaction to coming to a laboratory, rather than a true baseline measure of cortisol activity, and suggest that testing done in the home may better identify true increases and recoveries in cortisol levels in response to stressors of interest. Also, cortisol varies throughout the day, with peak levels being reached upon awakening, and lowest levels found after sleep onset (deWeerth et al., 2003, Gunnar and Talge, 2008). This natural variability can introduce additional, nonsubstantive variability into cortisol assays, which is problematic given that most studies of children's cortisol responses to stress have tested participants at different times of day.

With these issues in mind, we examined whether a significant cortisol increase could be elicited from preschool-aged children using a task adapted from Lewis and Ramsay (2002). This task, which already incorporated many features advocated by Gunnar et al. (2009) for use in tasks to elicit cortisol from children, was further adapted to be suitable for use with younger children, and to incorporate additional elements implicated by Dickerson and Kemeny (2004). Additionally, we attempted to control or eliminate nonsubstantive influences (e.g., time of day) on children's cortisol.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were an unselected community sample of 215 three-year-old children from southwestern Ontario, who were recruited for a larger study of genetic and other biological and contextual influences on child temperament and psychopathology risk. Children were recruited by contacting families through a university's developmental research participant pool and by advertisements placed in local daycares, preschools, recreational facilities, and on websites. Children with significant medical or

Data analysis techniques

As is typically found (Gunnar and Talge, 2008), our cortisol values were positively skewed. A log 10 transformation of the raw cortisol values yielded unskewed cortisol values that were used in all analyses.

Table 1 presents correlations between mean cortisol levels at each sampling time and all major study variables. All correlations involving NE and PE were partial correlations controlling for baseline NE and PE values. As expected (Wellhoener et al., 2004), cortisol at 10, 20, 30, 40, and 50 

Discussion

Methodological issues may have hampered research on cortisol reactivity in young children, such that few studies have reported the expected pattern of reactivity in children's cortisol responses to a standardized stress paradigm. In other words, it is unusual to find a quadratic function in such data, despite the fact that this is what would be expected given the nature of the physiological stress response (Earle et al., 1999, Roy et al., 2001, Smeekens et al., 2007). Using the task and

Limitations and future directions

As we have noted, research aiming to elicit cortisol increases in children this age has met with limited success. Thus, our task and methods incorporated a wide array of features aimed at maximizing the likelihood that we would obtain a cortisol increase. We appear to have been successful in obtaining such an increase; however, a limitation of our findings is that it is unclear whether specific aspects of our paradigm played a more critical role than others in producing this response. Future

Role of the funding sources

This research was supported by grants from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR MOP86458), the Children's Health Research Institute (CHRI), and the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation. These funding agencies had no role in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of data, the writing of the report or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Conflict of interest

None declared.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Margaret Sullivan, Michael Lewis, and Nestor Lopez-Duran for their assistance in the development of the task and procedures, Robert Gardner and Lea Dougherty for their advice regarding data analysis, and Jim Staples for providing access to laboratory equipment.

References (34)

  • L.S. Aiken et al.

    Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions

    (1991)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Maternal and child contributions to cortisol response to emotional arousal in young children from low-income, rural communities

    Dev. Psychol.

    (2008)
  • S.S. Dickerson et al.

    Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2004)
  • L.D. Dorn et al.

    Salivary cortisol reflects serum cortisol: analysis of circadian profiles

    Ann. Clin. Biochem.

    (2007)
  • L.M. Dunn et al.

    Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test

    (1997)
  • B.L. Fredrickson et al.

    What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001

    J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.

    (2003)
  • M.R. Gunnar et al.

    Neuroendocrine measures in developmental research

  • Cited by (50)

    • Prefrontal cortical thickness mediates the association between cortisol reactivity and executive function in childhood

      2020, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, it is also possible that the different tasks used to elicit stress response at the preschool-age and school-age assessments may have differences in sensitivity and reliability. Both tasks had similar stress-inducing components of social evaluation and inability to complete the task, which reliably evoke a cortisol response in children and adults (Gunnar et al., 2009; Kryski et al., 2011); but it is possible that one of the assessments may have induced a larger or different cortisol response. Future studies should aim to replicate this finding and further examine the potential of school-age cortisol reactivity, rather than preschool-age cortisol reactivity, predicting executive function.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text