Testosterone responses to competition predict future aggressive behaviour at a cost to reward in men

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.018Get rights and content

Summary

The extent to which trait factors (baseline testosterone concentrations, trait dominance) and state factors (change in social status, change in testosterone concentrations) would predict reactive aggression in a subsequent task that involved provocation was examined in 99 participants (39 men and 60 women). Participants first competed in same-sex dyads on a Number Tracing Task for which the outcome (win or loss) was rigged. After the competition, participants performed the Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP), a behavioural measure of reactive aggression against an opponent (actually a computer program). Trait dominance predicted baseline testosterone in men, but not women, and men made more aggressive responses than did women. Baseline testosterone concentrations did not predict aggressive behaviour in either men or women. Winners and losers did not differ in competition-induced change in testosterone. However, change in testosterone concentrations predicted aggressive responses in the PSAP for men in the loss condition, and aggressive responses were made at a cost to obtaining reward points. For men in the win condition, aggressive responses were predicted by an interaction between trait dominance and change in testosterone concentrations. These findings suggest that situational changes in testosterone concentrations modulate future aggressive behaviour in men.

Introduction

The World Health Organization has estimated that for every death due to physical aggression, another 20–40 youth require hospital treatment for an aggression-related injury (Mercy et al., 2002). The variety of ways in which aggressive behaviour is manifested (e.g., “road rage”, bullying, child abuse, domestic abuse and workplace violence) indicates the multifaceted nature of this behaviour. Despite the negative consequences of aggressive behaviour, the use (or threat) of aggression can be beneficial under certain conditions (e.g., athletic competition, self-defense, derogation of same-sex rivals and establishment of status hierarchies). Psychobiological investigations of the factors contributing to the expression of aggressive behaviour have identified many of the individual differences and situational factors that are associated with aggression, although most investigations in people have relied on self-report measures (see reviews by Anderson and Bushman, 2002, Bettencourt et al., 2006, Trainor and Nelson, 2007).

Dominance is a personality trait that involves the desire to seek control and/or influence over social situations, events, and relationships (Mehrabian, 1996). Although trait dominance is theoretically and empirically related to aggression, there have been few studies of the relationship between the two variables (Bettencourt et al., 2006). Individual differences in trait dominance predicted trait aggression as measured by self-report (Archer and Webb, 2006, Johnson et al., 2007), and men tend to score higher than women on self-report measures of trait dominance (Budaev, 1999, Costa et al., 2001) and on several measures of aggression (Archer, 2004). Given the empirical relationship between trait dominance and self-report aggression, it is plausible that trait dominance would also be related to behavioural aggression.

Testosterone is a biological factor of relevance to aggressive behaviour and to dominance in many species (reviewed in Simon and Lu, 2006, Trainor and Nelson, 2007). There have been reports of a positive association between self-report trait dominance and baseline testosterone concentrations (Cashdan, 1995, Grant and France, 2001, Sellers et al., 2007), although others have failed to replicate this finding (see Josephs et al., 2006, Stanton and Schultheiss, 2007). The relationship between baseline testosterone concentrations and various forms of aggressive behaviour is less evident in studies of people than in other animals (Book et al., 2001, Archer et al., 2005). The inconsistent findings for aggression may be due, in part, to the use of self-report measures as opposed to the direct measurement of aggressive behaviour (but see Pope et al., 2000, Klinesmith et al., 2006). Further, dynamic fluctuations in testosterone concentrations may be more related to aggressive behaviour than are baseline testosterone concentrations (Hermans et al., 2008). We recently found that baseline testosterone concentrations did not predict aggressive behaviour, but that aggressive behaviour was positively correlated with a rise in testosterone (Carré and McCormick, 2008). This result mirrors the findings of a study in non-human primates in which baseline testosterone concentrations did not predict aggressive behaviour, but aggressive behaviour was positively associated with a rise in testosterone concentrations (Ross et al., 2004).

Social interactions are known to modulate testosterone concentrations. For instance, winning competitive interactions (reviewed in Mazur and Booth, 1998, Archer, 2006, van Anders and Watson, 2006), good individual athletic performance (Edwards et al., 2006), the vicarious experience of victory and defeat (Bernhardt et al., 1998), and interactions with an attractive member of the opposite sex (Roney et al., 2003, Roney et al., 2007) all lead to changes in salivary testosterone concentrations. Dynamic shifts in testosterone concentrations have been proposed to influence future competitive and/or aggressive behaviours (Wingfield et al., 1990, Mazur, 1985, Mazur and Booth, 1998). A few studies have directly tested this hypothesis. For example, among losers (but not winners) of a competition, men whose testosterone concentrations had risen were more likely to choose to compete again, whereas men whose testosterone concentrations decreased chose the non-competitive option (Mehta and Josephs, 2006). We have also shown that changes in testosterone concentrations and aggressive behaviour during a competition predicted subsequent choice of a novel competitive task over a non-competitive task (Carré and McCormick, 2008). Furthermore, experimental studies have demonstrated that exogenous testosterone administrations increased cardiac responses to angry faces (van Honk et al., 2001), decreased fear-potentiated startle (Hermans et al., 2006a), increased visuospatial performance (Aleman et al., 2004), increased subcortical responses to angry faces (Hermans et al., 2008), decreased empathetic behaviour (Hermans et al., 2006b), and decreased conscious detection of angry faces (van Honk and Schutter, 2007). Although these studies support the idea that situational or experimental changes in testosterone concentrations are functionally related to future social behaviours, they do not speak to the issue of whether such changes in testosterone concentrations predict future aggressive behaviour.

Evidence from animal models suggests that the relationship between testosterone concentrations and future aggression is causal. A study of castrated male mice on low testosterone replacement found that those receiving a testosterone injection after a successful aggressive encounter were more aggressive in subsequent encounters compared to those that received a saline injection after a successful aggressive encounter (Trainor et al., 2004). One study has investigated the influence of a situation-specific change in salivary testosterone concentrations on future aggressive behaviour in people by comparing men who were given the opportunity to interact with a toy gun or a board game (Klinesmith et al., 2006). Men who interacted with the toy gun were more aggressive (as defined by the amount of hot sauce placed in another’s drink) compared to men who interacted with the board game. Importantly, the relationship between type of interaction and extent of aggressive behaviour was mediated by a rise in salivary testosterone concentrations, suggesting that testosterone was a factor influencing aggressive behaviour.

The studies above show relationships between either trait factors and aggressive behaviour or state factors and aggressive behaviour. The General Aggression Model (GAM) (Anderson and Bushman, 2002) posits that trait/personological factors (including personality traits, gender, attitudes and genetic predispositions) and state/situational factors (including features of the situation or environment such as the presentation of provocation, aggression cues, level of frustration and pain) influence various cognitive, emotional, metabolic and arousal mechanisms that mediate aggressive behaviour. However, studies of how trait and state factors interact to predict aggressive behaviour are lacking. We tested the hypothesis, derived from the literature reviewed above, that a competition-induced change in testosterone concentrations would predict subsequent aggressive behaviour as measured using the Point Subtraction Aggression Paradigm (PSAP). We included trait dominance as an individual difference variable and tested how this variable was associated with testosterone concentrations. Furthermore, based on previous self-report studies (Archer and Webb, 2006, Johnson et al., 2007), we predicted that trait dominance would be positively related to aggressive behaviour. Based on a few previous studies (Grant and France, 2001, Sellers et al., 2007), we also predicted that individual differences in baseline testosterone concentrations would be positively related to trait dominance. We included gender as a variable in our analyses because although men have higher concentrations of testosterone, are more physically aggressive (Archer, 2004), and have higher trait dominance scores (Budaev, 1999, Costa et al., 2001), the research literature is equivocal as to whether the relationships among these variables might differ for men and women (Dabbs and Hargrove, 1997, Mazur et al., 1997, Bateup et al., 2002, Kivlighan et al., 2005, Edwards et al., 2006, Josephs et al., 2006, Mehta et al., 2008).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Canisius College Psychology Department, and all procedures were approved by the Canisius College Institutional Review Board. The sample consisted of 39 men (mean age = 19.51, S.D. = 2.86) and 60 women (mean age = 18.88, S.D. = 1.03). An additional two men and two women were not included in the sample because they were taking prescription medications (ritalin, antidepressants and thyroxin).

Trait dominance questionnaire

Participants first completed a brief 10-item questionnaire assessing trait

Descriptive statistics and simple correlations

Descriptive statistics for the trait measures (basal testosterone concentrations and trait dominance score) and PSAP responses are presented in Table 1. Outcome (win–loss) was included as a factor in the initial analyses as a manipulation check of the random assignment, and it was never a significant factor. The expected sex differences were observed: men had higher baseline testosterone concentrations (F1, 73 = 81.19, p < 0.001) and higher trait dominance scores (F1, 97 = 6.25, p = 0.01) than women.

Discussion

The major finding from the current investigation is that testosterone concentrations after a competitive interaction predicted future reactive aggression in men and not women. Notably, men were more aggressive than women, supporting the general finding of higher direct aggression among men compared to women (see Archer, 2004). Furthermore, there was a significant positive association between baseline testosterone concentrations and trait dominance in men but not in women. Overall, these

Role of funding sources

This research was supported by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to CMM and a NSERC Canada Graduate Scholarship to JMC. The funding sources had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest for any of the authors.

Acknowledgments

We thank Kerendu Wombosa and Megan Kontrimas for their help with data collection and Professor Nancy DeCourville for statistical advice.

References (67)

  • D. Edwards et al.

    Intercollegiate soccer: saliva cortisol and testosterone are elevated during competition, and testosterone is related to status and social connectedness with teammates

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2006)
  • G. Gerra et al.

    Aggressive responding of male heroin addicts under methadone treatment: psychometric and neuroendocrine correlates

    Drug Alcohol Depend.

    (2001)
  • L.R. Goldberg et al.

    The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures

    J. Res. Personal.

    (2006)
  • W. Goymann et al.

    Distinguishing seasonal androgen responses from male-male androgen responsiveness—revisiting the challenge hypothesis

    Horm. Behav.

    (2007)
  • V. Grant et al.

    Dominance and testosterone in women

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2001)
  • E. Hermans et al.

    A single administration of testosterone reduces fear-potentiated startle in humans

    Biol. Psychiat.

    (2006)
  • E. Hermans et al.

    Testosterone administration reduces empathetic behavior: a facial mimicry study

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2006)
  • E. Hermans et al.

    Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans

    Biol. Psychiat.

    (2008)
  • N. Hess et al.

    Sex differences in indirect aggression: Psychological evidence from young adults

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (2006)
  • K.T. Kivlighan et al.

    Gender differences in testosterone and cortisol response to competition

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2005)
  • A. Mazur et al.

    Sex difference in testosterone response to a video game contest

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (1997)
  • P. Mehta et al.

    Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to compete again

    Horm. Behav.

    (2006)
  • M. Newman et al.

    Testosterone, cognition and social status

    Horm. Behav.

    (2005)
  • J. Roney et al.

    Rapid endocrine responses of young men to social interactions with young women

    Horm. Behav.

    (2007)
  • J. Roney et al.

    Behavioural and hormonal responses of men to brief interactions with women

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (2003)
  • C. Ross et al.

    Intensity of aggressive interactions modulates testosterone in male marmosets

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2004)
  • O. Schultheiss et al.

    Implicit power motivation moderates men’s testosterone responses to imagined and real dominance success

    Horm. Behav.

    (1999)
  • O. Schultheiss et al.

    Implicit power motivation predicts men’s testosterone changes and implicit learning in a contest situation

    Horm. Behav.

    (2002)
  • J. Sellers et al.

    Hormones and personality: testosterone as a marker of individual differences

    J. Res. Personal.

    (2007)
  • S. Stanton et al.

    Basal and dynamic relationship between implicit power motivation and estradiol in women

    Horm. Behav.

    (2007)
  • B. Trainor et al.

    Opposing hormonal mechanisms of aggression revealed through short-lived testosterone manipulations and multiple winning experiences

    Horm. Behav.

    (2004)
  • T. Allen et al.

    A study of male and female aggressive responding under conditions providing an escape option

    Psychol. Rec.

    (1996)
  • C. Anderson et al.

    Human aggression

    Ann. Rev. Psychol.

    (2002)
  • Cited by (151)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Tel.: +1 905 688 5550x3109.

    2

    Tel.: +1 716 888 2895.

    View full text