Elsevier

Psychiatry Research

Volume 245, 30 November 2016, Pages 27-35
Psychiatry Research

“Not Just Right Experiences” as a psychological endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from an Italian family study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.005Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The heart of OCD may be the subject's underlying impression that “something is wrong”.

  • Not Just Right Experiences may be a putative endophenotype of obsessional symptoms.

  • The offspring of parents with OC symptoms reported more NJREs than controls.

  • NJREs as an endophenotype is discussed also in reference to NIMH RDoC.

Abstract

The heart of the obsessional process may be considered the subject's underlying impression that “something is wrong” or “that something is not just as it should be”. This phenomenon, labeled “not just right experiences” (NJREs), has increasingly been receiving attention as a possible marker of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). The present study sought to add to the evidence that NJREs may be a putative endophenotype of obsessional symptoms. To this aim, measures of NJREs, obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptoms and psychological distress were compared in offspring of parents with and without OC symptoms. The offspring of parents with OC symptoms (N=120) reported higher frequency and severity of NJREs compared to offspring of parents without OC symptoms (N=106). Such differences remained significant for NJREs frequency and close to significance for NJREs severity, when general distress (i.e., anxiety and depression) was controlled. The possible role of NJREs as an endophenotype for OCD is discussed in reference to Gottesman and Gould criteria and the National Institute of Mental Health RDoC initiative.

Introduction

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD) is a serious mental condition characterized by the presence of persistent, intrusive, and distressing obsessions or compulsions, with marked impairment in quality of life (Eisen et al., 2006, Kugler et al., 2013, Parkin, 1997). Obsessions are uncontrolled, unpleasant and unwanted thoughts, impulses or images accompanied by a feeling of urgency or catastrophe, leading to repetitive, time-consuming or ritualistic behaviors (compulsions). OCD represents one of the most incapacitating psychiatric disorders owing to its intensity, the continuous and unchanging or deteriorative course of its symptoms, and the disturbance in psychosocial functioning that they cause (Albert et al., 2010, Mancebo et al., 2008, Visser et al., 2014).

OCD is characterized by a clinical diversity reflected in a wide range of symptoms, and the clinical presentation can vary both within and across patients over time. Actually, many studies have provided strong evidence that OCD is clinically heterogeneous and that this clinical heterogeneity is likely due to etiologic heterogeneity (e.g., Abramowitz and Jacoby, 2015, Bloch et al., 2008, McKay et al., 2004).

Behavioral genetic (twin) research shows that OCD arises from a mix of genetic and environmental factors, but much remains to be learned about the nature of these etiologic influences (e.g., Pauls et al., 2014). In particular, recent genetic studies demonstrated that OCD is associated with multiple genes, with most having a modest association with OCD, suggesting a polygenic model in which multiple genes make small, incremental contributions to the risk of developing the disorder (e.g., Taylor, 2012, Taylor and Jang, 2011; see also Stewart et al., 2013 on the null results in genome-wide association study of OCD).

Unfortunately, little is known about the predictors and moderators of response to therapies in OCD, despite extensive research on this topic (see review by Knopp et al., 2013).

Like all psychiatric disorders, gaining a complete understanding of what comprises OCD and the underlying etiological mechanisms will probably require a change in how the disorder is conceptualized. In particular, the notion of endophenotype has been proposed as a means of facilitating research into the etiology of complex and heterogeneous disorders such as OCD. Endophenotypes may be considered intermediary between etiologic factors (bottom-level) and phenotypic manifestations (top-level) of a given disorder (see Gottesman and Gould, 2003, Kendler and Neale, 2010). An endophenotype may be a variable from any of a number of levels of organization, including biochemical, neurophysiological, neuroanatomical, cognitive, or other variables, including those obtained from self-report measures (Cannon and Keller, 2006, Gottesman and Gould, 2003). Importantly, an endophenotype can be a risk factor for a disorder even if the person is not currently symptomatic; in addition, it should be found in unaffected family members at a higher rate than in the general population (for a broad discussion about the role of endophenotypes in psychopathology, see Hasler et al., 2004).

In the last few years, several scholars have considered the lack of a sense of satisfaction and presence of feelings of doubt as central features of obsessions and associated compulsions. In fact, the heart of the obsessional process seems the subject's underlying impression that “something is wrong”. In other words, obsessions may be thought of as the perception of a mistake and/or error in certain behavioral situations (see Sica et al., 2015). Based on various empirical data, the construct of “not just right experiences” (NJREs; Coles et al., 2003) has been proposed as a candidate endophenotype for OCD (see also Miguel et al., 2005).

What evidence supports NJREs as an endophenotype? First, the cross-sectional association between NJREs and OCD or obsessive-compulsive (OC) symptoms appears robust for both in nonclinical and clinical populations (e.g., Ferrão et al., 2012, Ghisi et al., 2010, Ghisi et al., 2013; Taylor et al., 2012). In addition, Coles et al. (2012) investigated patients’ reports of various factors that may have played a role in the transition from the presence of obsessions and compulsions to full-blown OCD. Increases in the strength of urges for things to feel ‘just right’ were commonly viewed as contributing to the onset of OCD. Importantly, associations between sensory phenomena (i.e., NJREs) and OCD were also noted in a pediatric OCD population (Lewin et al., 2015).

Second, experimental studies have demonstrated that NJREs elicited in the laboratory predicted OC symptoms. For instance, in a sample of undergraduate students, affective response to clutter (i.e., NJREs) was uniquely predictive of hand-washing duration when controlling for pre-wash anxiety (Cougle et al., 2011). Moreover, measures of incompleteness predicted urges to check following a stove checking task (Cougle et al., 2013; see also, Summers et al., 2014). Buse et al. (2014) assessed reaction times (RT) to harmonic and disharmonic chord sequences as well as their emotional appraisal in 64 healthy young adults. The participants clearly indicated that disharmonic chord sequences sounded not just right and incomplete and rated them as unpleasant, arousing, and irritating. This effect tended to be greater among participants who reported stronger general experiences of incompleteness as an underlying core dimension of OCD-like symptoms.

Third, in a one-year longitudinal study on undergraduates, NJREs predicted OC symptom variation even when looming style (a specific cognitive mechanism for anxiety, Rachman et al., 2005) was accounted for (Sica et al., 2012).

Lastly, NJREs appear specific to OCD. In several studies, NJREs were significantly more strongly correlated with OC symptoms than other domains of psychopathology (e.g., social anxiety, worry, depression; Coles et al., 2003, Coles et al., 2005; Ecker and Gönner, 2008; Ghisi et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2012). Sica et al. (2015) who found that a group of OCD patients reported higher levels of NJRE severity than groups of patients with either gambling (GD) or eating disorders (ED) recently corroborated this evidence. In the same study, while a measure of NJREs did not discriminate between OCD patients and patients with hair-pulling disorder, this last group did not have higher scores of NJREs severity than GD and ED counterparts.

It is not surprising therefore that the DSM-5 newly acknowledged NJREs as one of the “affective responses” seen in OCD (American Psychiatric Association, APA, 2013, p. 239). Despite the substantial evidence that indicate NJREs as a possible mechanism involved in OCD, no published studies have investigated whether NJREs are present in relatives of individuals with OCD or OC symptoms (unaffected family members) at a higher rate than in the general population. Providing data about this important issue would strengthen the evidence for NJREs as a putative endophenotype for OCD and OC symptoms.

We speculated that the frequency and/or severity of NJREs should be higher in offspring of parents with OC symptoms compared to offspring of parents without OC symptoms. This supposition is in fact coherent with the idea that NJREs might be a liability factor for the OCD. To test our hypothesis, measures of NJREs, OC symptoms, anxiety, and depression were collected from a sample of college students and their parents. Then, we identified two groups of college students: those with at least one parent with some OC symptoms (as measured by the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory, see below) and those with no parents with OC symptoms. The first group of students was considered an “at-risk group” for OC symptoms whereas the second one was considered not at risk for OC symptoms.

We chose a nonclinical sample because we believe that, in case of OCD, it has certain advantages compared to clinical counterparts. For example, those seeking treatment for OCD represent a minority of the OCD population (Grabe et al., 2000), and likely differ from non-help seekers on social, economic, attitudinal, and personality factors. Confounding factors such as prior treatment types and treatment effects and, above all, comorbidity, also pose challenges for studies of OC phenomena in clinical populations. In addition, OCD is a chronic disorder, especially if not treated in the appropriate way (Sica et al., 2010): we do not know how a chronic OCD affects the psychological (and neurobiological) functions of people in the long-term. Also, since OCD occurs in only 2–3% of the population, it can be time intensive and costly to recruit clinical samples of an adequate size.

On the other hand, OC symptoms do occur in the general population (e.g., Adam et al., 2012, de Bruijn et al., 2010; Grabe et al., 2000), allowing researchers to recruit larger samples with relative convenience. As a matter of fact, researchers have pursued various forms of analogue research in order to study OC phenomena. Let us consider the evidences in favor of such choice.

  • 1)

    Studies of analogue samples (i.e., student and community participants) highlight the prevalence of subclinical OC symptoms. According to surveys, up to 90% of people report that they at least occasionally experience intrusive thoughts that are similar in form and content to clinical obsessions (e.g, Clark, 1992, Freeston et al., 1991). Several studies of student or community samples have reported the prevalence of “caseness” as assessed by the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R; Foa et al., 2002) (e.g., Cuttler and Taylor, 2012, Kaczkurkin, 2013). Therefore, OC symptoms are present to some degree in the non-clinical samples, providing initial evidence for a continuum of severity.

  • 2)

    Distress, impairment, and reduced quality of life are also part of the diagnostic criteria for OCD (APA, 2013). Accordingly, studies also reveal some degree of impairment and treatment seeking even among non-clinical groups, and thus support the dimensional model of OC symptoms (Adam et al., 2012, de Bruijn et al., 2010; Grabe et al., 2000). Even more important, two taxometric studies have found support for a dimensional latent structure for OC symptoms globally (Haslam et al., 2005, Olatunji et al., 2008).

  • 3)

    A large body of research indicates that for the most part, the themes, content, and form of intrusive distressing (obsession-like) thoughts are the same among non-clinical (analogue) and clinically diagnosed OCD samples (e.g., Belloch et al., 2004; García-Soriano et al., 2011; Julien et al., 2009, Rasmussen and Eisen, 1989). Moreover, Rachman and de Silva (1978) found that even trained clinicians could not distinguish between the content of obsession-like intrusive thoughts reported by people with and without a diagnosis of OCD. Likewise, most types of compulsions observed in clinical OCD samples (e.g., checking, counting, repeating, washing) are also observed in nonclinical samples (Flament et al., 1988, Henderson and Pollard, 1988).

  • 4)

    Numerous studies have used factor and cluster analysis of OC symptom measures to elucidate the thematic structure of OC symptoms in non-clinical and clinical samples. In general, findings indicate structural invariance between clinical and non-clinical samples across different measures of OC symptoms (Maudsley Obsessional Compulsive Inventory; Hodgson and Rachman, 1977; Padua Inventory, Sanavio, 1988, van Oppen et al., 1995; OCI-R; Foa et al., 2002, Overduin and Furnham, 2012; Schedule of Obsessions Compulsions and Pathological Impulses, Watson and Wu (2005); Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale; Goodman et al., 1989a, Goodman et al., 1989b, Katerberg et al., 2010, Wu et al., 2007, López-Solà et al., 2014).

  • 5)

    A recent meta-analysis compared the pattern of results from twin studies in which one or both twins were diagnosed with OCD versus twin studies of non-clinical samples in which twins completed measures of OC symptom severity (Taylor, 2011). Results indicated that all the findings from clinical samples were replicated in non-clinical samples (see also Taylor et al., 2010). These findings suggest that OC symptoms, as assessed in analogue samples, are etiologically related to the diagnosis of OCD.

  • 6)

    Psychological processes and mechanisms hypothesized to govern the development and maintenance of OC symptoms do overlap across types of samples. This is true both for NJREs (as previously illustrated) and for alternative models of OCD development. Regarding the latter, for instance, researchers have identified six domains of dysfunctional beliefs (i.e., “obsessive beliefs”) thought to be associated to OC phenomena. Numerous correlational, prospective, and experimental studies demonstrate that obsessive beliefs are present and associated with OC symptom severity in both clinical and non-clinical analogue samples (e.g., Abramowitz and Deacon, 2006, Abramowitz et al., 2006, Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group, 2003, Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group, 2005).

  • 7)

    Various experimental paradigms have found the presence of attentional biases in individuals with OCD and in analogue samples. In a meta-analytic study, Bar-Haim et al. (2007), examined 50 studies using samples of non-clinical individuals: they found that a threat-related attentional bias is an overlapping feature of individuals with clinical and nonclinical levels of OC symptoms (see also Coles et al., 2006, Dek et al., 2010, van den Hout and Kindt, 2003).

  • 8)

    Contemporary treatment development continues to rely on analogue samples for the initial evaluation of potentially effective interventions (e.g., Amir et al., 2009, Najmi and Amir, 2010). Analogue treatment research is also useful for answering important scientific questions such as the process of change, the specific efficacy of different treatment components, and the manner in which treatments might be modified to optimally target different OCD symptom presentations. For example, Cougle et al. (2007) found that the process and outcome of exposure to a feared contaminant differed according to whether non-clinical analogue OC participants were concerned with illness or other threats. In contrast to participants with illness-related concerns, those with non-illness concerns (e.g., loss of control) did not experience a reduction in the urge to wash following a single session of exposure therapy (see also, Olatunji et al., 2009).

To conclude, research supports the idea that OC-related phenomena among non-clinical analogue samples are milder variants of those observed among individuals with symptoms severe enough to meet the criteria for a diagnosis of OCD (for a review, see Abramowitz and Jacoby, 2014).

Section snippets

Participants and procedure

A portion of the current sample participated in a previous study of the familiarity of NJREs (see, Sica et al., 2013). Approximately 600 undergraduates of Health Sciences (Psychology and Medicine) and Sciences (Computer Science and Engineering) from two different Italian universities in Central and Northern Italy were approached during lectures by faculty to participate in a study “on the psychology of family relationships”. To be eligible for the study, individuals had to have been raised by

Preliminary analyses

Eight-seven percent of the ARO group and 74% of the COs reported having experienced at least one of the 10 NJREs described in the NJRE-Q-R. In Table 3 the percentage of individuals who reported having experienced the NJREs listed in the NJRE-Q-R are displayed. For ARO individuals the most common were, “When talking to people, I have had the sensation that my words did not sound just right” (63%), “I have had the sensation after getting dressed that part of my clothes (tags, collars, pant legs,

Discussion

OCD is yet to be fully understood, especially in its etiologic features; the high heterogeneity of this disorder represents a challenge for both researchers and clinicians (e.g., Leckman et al., 2010, Summerfeldt et al., 2014). The boundary of OC manifestations is a controversial issue (e.g., Abramowitz and Jacoby, 2015, Lochner and Stein, 2006, Phillips et al., 2010); moreover, the treatment of OCD produces less than optimal results in a significant number of cases (Sica et al., 2010). We are

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Gail Steketee for advice on revision of the manuscript.

References (106)

  • J. Cougle et al.

    A multimethod examination of the role of incompleteness in compulsive checking

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2013)
  • J.R. Cougle et al.

    Mechanisms of change in erp treatment of compulsive hand washing: does primary threat make a difference?

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2007)
  • B.J. Deacon et al.

    The effects of safety behaviors on the fear of contamination:an experimental investigation

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2008)
  • E.P. Dek et al.

    Repeated checking causes distrust in memory but not in attention and perception

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2010)
  • W. Ecker et al.

    Incompleteness and harm avoidance in OCD symptom dimensions

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2008)
  • J. Eisen et al.

    Impact of obsessive-compulsive disorder on quality of life

    Compr. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • Y.A. Ferrão et al.

    Sensory Phenomena associated with repetitive behaviors in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an exploratory study of 1001 patients

    Psychiatry Res

    (2012)
  • M.F. Flament et al.

    Obsessive compulsive disorder in adolescence: an epidemiological study

    J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry

    (1988)
  • M.H. Freeston et al.

    Cognitive intrusions in a non-clinical population: i. response style, subjective experience, and appraisal

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1991)
  • G. García-Soriano et al.

    Symptom dimensions in obsessive–compulsive disorder: from normal cognitive intrusions to clinical obsessions

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2011)
  • M. Ghisi et al.

    In search of specificity: “not just right experiences” and obsessive–compulsive symptoms in non-clinical and clinical Italian individuals

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2010)
  • N. Haslam et al.

    subtyping obsessive compulsive disorder: a taxometric analysis

    Behav. Ther.

    (2005)
  • R.J. Hodgson et al.

    Obsessional-compulsive complaints

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1977)
  • M. van den Hout et al.

    Phenomenological validity of an OCD-memory model and the remember/know distinction

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2003)
  • A.N. Kaczkurkin

    The effect of manipulating task difficulty on error-related negativity in individuals with obsessive–compulsive symptoms

    Biol. Psychol.

    (2013)
  • J. Knopp et al.

    A systematic review of predictors and moderators of response to psychological therapies in OCD: Do we have enough empirical evidence to target treatment?

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2013)
  • B. Kugler et al.

    Quality of life in obsessive-compulsive disorder: the role of mediating variables

    Psychiatry Res

    (2013)
  • C. Lochner et al.

    Does work on obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders contribute to understanding the heterogeneity of obsessive–compulsive disorder?

    Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • C. López-Solà et al.

    Spanish version of the Dimensional Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (DOCS): psychometric properties and relation to obsessive beliefs

    Compr. Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • M. Mancebo et al.

    Correlates of occupational disability in a clinical sample of obsessive-compulsive disorder

    Compr. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • D. McKay et al.

    A critical evaluation of obsessive–compulsive disorder subtypes: symptoms versus mechanisms

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2004)
  • B.O. Olatunji et al.

    Differential habituation of fear and disgust during repeated exposure to threat-relevant stimuli in contamination-based OCD: an analogue study

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2009)
  • M.K. Overduin et al.

    Assessing obsessive–compulsive disorder: a review of self-report measures

    J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord.

    (2012)
  • S. Rachman et al.

    Abnormal and normal obsessions

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1978)
  • E. Rassin et al.

    Thought–action fusion as a causal factor in the development of intrusions

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1999)
  • M. Russo et al.

    Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a “sensory-motor” problem?

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (2014)
  • E. Sanavio

    Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1988)
  • C. Sica et al.

    “Not just right experiences” predict obsessive–compulsive symptoms in non-clinical italian individuals: a one-year longitudinal study

    J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord.

    (2012)
  • C. Sica et al.

    The Italian version of the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory: its psychometric properties on community and clinical samples

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2009)
  • C. Sica et al.

    “Not just right experiences” are specific to obsessive–compulsive disorder: further evidence from italian clinical samples

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2015)
  • C. Sica et al.

    Fathers’ “not just right experiences” predict obsessive–compulsive symptoms in their sons: family study of a non-clinical italian sample

    J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord.

    (2013)
  • L. Summerfeldt et al.

    Symptom structure in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a confirmatory factor-analytic study

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (1999)
  • L. Summerfeldt et al.

    Examining an obsessive-compulsive core dimensions model: structural validity of harm avoidance and incompleteness

    J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord.

    (2014)
  • B. Summers et al.

    Visual, tactile, and auditory “not just right” experiences: associations with obsessive-compulsive symptoms and perfectionism

    Behav. Ther.

    (2014)
  • S. Taylor

    Endophenotypes of obsessive–compulsive disorder: current status and future directions

    J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord.

    (2012)
  • J. Abramowitz et al.

    Obsessive-compulsive disorder in the DSM‐5

    Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract.

    (2014)
  • J. Abramowitz et al.

    Obsessive-compulsive and related disorders: a critical review of the new diagnostic class

    Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol.

    (2015)
  • Y. Adam et al.

    Obsessive–compulsive disorder in the community: 12-month prevalence, comorbidity and impairment

    Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol.

    (2012)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (2013)
  • Y. Bar-Haim et al.

    Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2007)
  • Cited by (15)

    • Are “not just-right experiences” trait and/or state marker for obsessive-compulsive disorder?

      2022, European Journal of Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      The authors concluded that NJREs could be a candidate endophenotypic marker in OCD. Although both studies included large samples, they included non-clinical participants evaluated on self-report without clinical interviews.13 This is the first study investigating NJREs in relatives of OCD patients.

    • Action inhibition in individuals with high obsessive-compulsive trait of incompleteness: An ERP study

      2021, Biological Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The absence of action completion signals is hence assumed to result in inappropriate action repetitions. Several authors have proposed that feelings of incompleteness and Not Just Right Experiences (NJREs) constitute a trait-like marker for vulnerability to OCD and exist on a spectrum from unobtrusive non-clinical manifestations, through mildly experienced discomfort, to pathological presentations (Belloch et al., 2016; Ben-Sasson, Dickstein, Lazarovich, & Ayalon, 2017; Sica, Bottesi, Caudek, Orsucci, & Ghisi, 2016; Summerfeldt et al., 2014). Ghisi, Bottesi, Sica, Sanavio, and Freeston (2013)) investigated the relationship between NJREs, uncomfortable feelings of actions and their perceptual consequences “just not being right”, and performance of patients with OCD and healthy controls on a Go/NoGo task.

    • Cognitive inflexibility specificity for individuals with high levels of obsessive-compulsive symptoms

      2020, Journal of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      NJREs are a common experience (Coles, Heimberg, Frost, & Steketee, 2005), with approximately 95% of non-clinical samples reporting at least one NJRE in their lifetime (Coles, Frost, Heimberg, & Rhéaume, 2003; Fergus, 2014). NJREs have been considered as a vulnerability factor for OCD symptoms, but with only a small-to-medium effect size – in other words, even if the level of NJRE is high, the likelihood of developing an OCD condition remains relatively small (Sica, Bottesi, Caudek, Orsucci, & Ghisi, 2016). In addition, it has been proposed that NJREs may be a putative endophenotype of obsessional symptoms: an endophenotype can be a risk factor for a disorder even if the person is not currently symptomatic (Sica et al., 2016).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text