Sluggish cognitive tempo and peer functioning in school-aged children: A six-month longitudinal study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.007Get rights and content

Abstract

Although research demonstrates sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) symptoms to be statistically distinct from other child psychopathologies (including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD], anxiety, depression, and oppositionality) and associated with social impairment, all studies conducted to date have been cross-sectional. Thus, while extant research demonstrates an association between SCT and social functioning, it is entirely unknown whether or not SCT longitudinally predicts increases in social impairment. This study provides an initial examination of the prospective association between SCT symptoms and children׳s peer functioning. Teachers of 176 children in 1st–6th grades (ages 6–13; 47% boys) provided ratings of children׳s psychopathology (i.e., SCT, ADHD, anxious/depressive, and oppositional/conduct problems) and peer functioning (i.e., popularity, negative social preference, peer impairment), and peer functioning was assessed again 6 months later. Multilevel modeling analyses indicated that, above and beyond child demographics, other psychopathologies, and baseline peer functioning, SCT symptoms were significantly associated with poorer peer functioning at the 6-month follow-up. In addition, 75% of children with high levels of SCT were rated as functionally impaired in the peer domain, in contrast to only 8% of children with low SCT. Further research is needed with larger samples to examine SCT over a longer developmental period and with other domains of adjustment.

Introduction

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) is defined by daydreaming, drowsiness, lethargy, mental confusion, and seeming to be in a world of one׳s own. Although initially identified as a set of symptoms hypothesized to be useful for identifying a distinct set of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Predominately Inattentive Type (ADHD-I) (McBurnett et al., 2001, Carlson and Mann, 2002), recent studies do not convincingly support the hypothesis that the presence of elevated SCT is useful for identifying a subset of children diagnosed with ADHD-I (Marshall et al., 2014, Willcutt et al., 2014). Still, SCT has recently gained attention for its broader relevance for child adjustment (Becker, 2013, Barkley, 2014, Becker et al., 2014b). In support of the increased attention devoted to the SCT construct, multiple studies using a variety of sample types and age ranges have demonstrated SCT to be statistically distinct from DSM-IV ADHD (Willcutt et al., 2012, Barkley, 2013) as well as symptoms of anxiety and depression (Burns et al., 2013, Becker et al., 2014a, Lee et al., 2014, Willcutt et al., 2014). Further, a growing body of research demonstrates that SCT is not only separable from ADHD and other psychopathologies but also related to a range of psychosocial impairments (see Barkley, 2014, Becker, 2013, for reviews). Given these findings, it has been suggested that SCT may be itself a distinct psychiatric disorder (Concentration Deficit Disorder; Barkley, 2014), although much more research is needed before determining precisely what role SCT should ultimately have in psychiatry, psychology, and developmental psychopathology.

One of the most consistent findings to date is that of an association between SCT and social impairment. Multiple studies have documented a significant relation between SCT symptoms and general social problems (Bauermeister et al., 2012, Becker and Langberg, 2013, Burns et al., 2013, Becker et al., 2014a, Lee et al., 2014, McBurnett et al., 2014, Willcutt et al., 2014). SCT symptoms have also been shown to be significantly associated with sensitivity to punishment broadly and shyness/fear specifically (Becker et al., 2013a). Other studies have shown that among youth with ADHD-I, those with high levels of SCT are more socially withdrawn (and less aggressive) than those without high SCT (Carlson and Mann, 2002, Marshall et al., 2014). In line with these findings, Willcutt and colleagues (2014) found SCT symptoms to be uniquely associated with social isolation after controlling for inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms. Finally, SCT symptoms were associated with a poorer perception of subtle social cues and less memory for a laboratory-based chat room conversation (Mikami et al., 2007).

Although these studies suggest that SCT is an important construct for understanding youth׳s social adjustment, a significant limitation of SCT-related research conducted to date is that all studies have used a cross-sectional design. Thus, while extant research demonstrates an association between SCT and social functioning, it is entirely unknown whether or not SCT longitudinally predicts increases in social impairment. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to provide an initial examination of the longitudinal association between SCT symptoms and children׳s peer functioning. Since the use of clinic-based samples of children diagnosed with ADHD make it “difficult to identify distinctive features that may be associated with SCT” (Barkley, 2013, p. 162), a non-referred school-based sample of children was used in the present study. Specifically, the teachers of children in first through sixth grades participated in a 6-month longitudinal study examining SCT in relation to subsequent peer functioning. In addition to being the first study to examine SCT as a longitudinal predictor of children׳s adjustment, multiple domains of peer functioning were examined (i.e., popularity, negative social preference, impairment in the peer domain). Since SCT is linked to social withdrawal, mental confusion, and slow processing, it was hypothesized that SCT symptoms would predict poorer peer functioning over a 6-month period, even after controlling for baseline peer functioning and other psychopathologies (i.e., ADHD inattentive, ADHD hyperactive-impulsive, anxious/depressive, and conduct/oppositional symptoms). Additional analyses were conducted that compared children with high levels of SCT to children with low levels of SCT, with the expectation that children with elevated SCT would have higher levels of other psychopathology symptoms and functional impairment in the peer domain than children without elevated SCT.

Section snippets

Participants

The current study included teacher ratings of 176 students attending an elementary school in the Midwestern United States. Students included in this study were in first through sixth grades (ages 6–13 at the fall time point, M=9.17, S.D.=1.82). The sample was approximately equally split between boys (n=82; 47%) and girls (n=94; 53%). According to official school records, and consistent with demographics of the surrounding community (95% White in the 2010 United States Census), the majority of

Correlation analyses

First, correlations among study variables at T1 were examined. Compared to their peers, children receiving free or reduced lunch had higher levels of both SCT symptoms (r=0.20, p=0.008) and ADHD inattentive symptoms (r=0.16, p=0.03). Boys and girls did not differ on teacher-reported internalizing symptoms, but boys were rated by teachers as displaying more SCT, inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, and conduct/oppositional symptoms than girls (rs=−0.17 to −0.23, all ps<0.05). Child age was

Discussion

To date, all studies examining the external validity of SCT have used a cross-sectional design, making longitudinal studies a clear research priority. This study makes an important contribution to the current literature by using a validated measure of SCT to examine whether SCT prospectively predicted peer functioning over a 6-month period in a sample of 176 school-aged children. Results from the present study indicated that, after controlling for the temporal stability of peer functioning,

Acknowledgment

The author thanks the teachers who participated in this study, as well as Aaron Luebbe for his assistance with study design and the undergraduate students who assisted with data collection.

References (30)

  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Reward and punishment sensitivity are differentially associated with ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in children

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2013)
  • R.B. Cairns et al.

    A brief method for assessing social development: structure, reliability, stability, and developmental validity of the Interpersonal Competence Scale

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1995)
  • R.A. Barkley

    Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and adolescents: executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2013)
  • R.A. Barkley

    Sluggish cognitive tempo (concentration deficit disorder?): current status, future directions, and a plea to change the name

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2014)
  • J.J. Bauermeister et al.

    Validity of the sluggish cognitive tempo, inattention, and hyperactivity symptom dimensions: neuropsychological and psychosocial correlates

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2012)
  • S.P. Becker

    Topical review: sluggish cognitive tempo: research findings and relevance for pediatric psychology

    Journal of Pediatric Psychology

    (2013)
  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Sluggish cognitive tempo among young adolescents with ADHD: relations to mental health, academic, and social functioning

    Journal of Attention Disorders

    (2013)
  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Clinical utility of the Vanderbilt ADHD Diagnostic Parent Rating Scale comorbidity screening scales

    Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics

    (2012)
  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Sluggish cognitive tempo in psychiatrically hospitalized children: factor structure and relations to internalizing symptoms, social problems, and observed behavioral dysregulation

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2014)
  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Sluggish cognitive tempo in abnormal child psychology: an historical overview and introduction to the special section

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2014)
  • S.P. Becker et al.

    Negative social preference in relation to internalizing symptoms among children with ADHD predominantly inattentive type: girls fare worse than boys

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2013)
  • G.L. Burns et al.

    Distinctions between sluggish cognitive tempo, ADHD-IN and depression symptom dimensions in Spanish first-grade children

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2013)
  • R.B. Cairns et al.

    Social structure and social cognition in early adolescence: affiliative patterns

    Journal of Early Adolescence

    (1985)
  • C.L. Carlson et al.

    Sluggish cognitive tempo predicts a different pattern of impairment in the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, predominantly inattentive type

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2002)
  • T.J. Dishion

    The peer context of troublesome child and adolescent behavior

  • Cited by (49)

    • Systematic Review: Assessment of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo Over the Past Decade

      2021, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text