Private self-consciousness in daily life: Relationships between rumination and reflection and well-being, and meaning in daily life

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.039Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study moved beyond trait reports of rumination, reflection, and meaning in life (presence and search) by examining within-person relationships between daily states of these constructs and well-being. Participants (N = 130) completed reports at the end of the day for 14 days. When analyzed together, daily rumination was negatively related to daily well-being whereas daily reflection was not (with one exception). In contrast, daily reflection was positively related to daily search for meaning in life, whereas rumination was not related to daily search for meaning in life. Reflection moderated the within-person relationships between rumination and well-being such that negative relationships between rumination and well-being were stronger at higher levels of reflection. In contrast, rumination had virtually no effect on search for meaning in life at higher levels of reflection. Lagged analyses found that daily reflection led to increases in daily positive deactivated affect (e.g., relaxation) and searching for meaning in life, and daily rumination led to increased presence of meaning in life the following day. These results highlight the importance of considering both reflection and rumination in studies of within-person variation and the value of considering within-person variability in understanding presence of and search for meaning in life.

Introduction

The present study concerned within-person (daily) variability in private self-consciousness, also referred to as self-focused attention. Much of the research on private self-consciousness has focused on this construct as a trait, and although such research is valuable, we believe private self-consciousness can also be understood as a state, an entity that varies within individuals. We based our conceptualization of private self-consciousness on the distinction between rumination and reflection introduced by Trapnell and Campbell (1999).

Trapnell and Campbell were motivated by what had come to be known as the “self-absorption paradox”: the incompatibility of research indicating that private self-consciousness is positively related to well-being and research indicating that it is negatively related. Trapnell and Campbell argued this could be due to the fact that private self-consciousness had separable components that could stem from different motives and have different outcomes. They proposed that private self-consciousness had two components: rumination, a neurotic self-attentiveness, and reflection, an intellectual self-attentiveness. They found that rumination was positively related to neuroticism, negative affect, and depression, whereas reflection was positively related to openness, need for cognition, and need for self-knowledge. Furthermore, they found that their measures of dispositional rumination and reflection were uncorrelated.

The present study assumed that the distinction between rumination and reflection is important not only at the between-person level but also at the within-person level. Such an assumption was based on the possibility that within-person relationships between constructs might be different from, and represent different processes than, between-person relationships between the same constructs (Affleck, Zautra, Tennen, & Armeli, 1999). We were also motivated by the possibility that daily measures might be less influenced than trait level measure by various types of bias (e.g. Nezlek, 2012, pp. 3–4). In the present study, participants provided measures describing their daily rumination, reflection, well-being, and meaning in life. Our analyses focused on relationships between daily rumination and reflection and daily well-being and meaning in life.

Previous studies about naturally occurring within-person variability in private self-consciousness have examined rumination but not reflection (e.g., Kashdan et al., 2012, Puterman et al., 2010) or have not distinguished the two (e.g., Nezlek, 2002). Nevertheless, this research has consistently found positive, same-day relationships between rumination and negative affect or negative events (e.g., Dickson et al., 2012, Genet and Siemer, 2012, Jose and Lim, 2015, Kashdan et al., 2012, Moberly and Watkins, 2008). Moreover, similar to research at the trait level, much of the research on daily rumination has examined relationships between rumination and negative predictors and outcomes. To our knowledge, only two studies have examined within-person relationships between rumination and positive outcomes, and each found a negative relationship between rumination and positive affect (Pe et al., 2013, Ruscio et al., 2015).

The present study complemented previous research by expanding well-being outcomes measures to include self-esteem, life satisfaction, and meaning in life, and by including a measure of daily reflection. Such an extension can be informative because affectively and non-affectively based measures of well-being, although related, assess different constructs (e.g., Nezlek, 2005). Drawing on trait level studies, we hypothesized that reflection would not be related to well-being as uniformly and negatively as rumination would be.

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to examine within-person relationships between private self-consciousness and meaning in life. Given the nature of both of these constructs, we thought such an examination would be fruitful. Meaning in life has generally been considered in terms of two distinct components, presence and search (Frankl, 1963, Steger et al., 2006). Presence has been defined as the extent to which one finds meaning, purpose, and coherence in life (Martela & Steger, 2016). Search has been defined as the “strength, intensity, and activity of people's desire and efforts to establish and/or augment their understanding of the meaning, significance, and purpose of their lives” (Steger, Kashdan, Sullivan, & Lorentz, 2008).

Implicit in both of these constructs is a sense that people examine, contemplate, and evaluate their lives, and such processes require thinking about the self. Consistent with this, Steger et al. (2008) found positive between-person relationships between search and rumination and reflection, and a negative between-person relationship between presence and rumination. The present study was designed to complement such research by examining within-person relationships between these constructs.

Participants in the present study answered a series of questions at the end of the day for two weeks. These questions consisted of measures of rumination and reflection, of presence and search for meaning in life, and of well-being, which included self-esteem, affect, and life satisfaction. Our analyses examined relationships between reflection/rumination and meaning in life and well-being. Based on previous research on within-person relationships, we expected daily rumination to be negatively related to daily well-being (i.e., positively related to daily negative affect and negatively related to positive affect, self-esteem, and life satisfaction). Based on trait level research we expected that reflection would not be strongly related to well-being if it would be related at all.

In terms of relationships with meaning in life, we expected rumination to be negatively related to presence of meaning in life. According to Park (2010), to feel that one's life has meaning (presence), requires that some type of meaning making process has occurred. Such meaning making processes are probably in progress (or halted) as people ruminate which means that people are less likely to find meaning in life when they are ruminating compared to when they are not. Given that Trapnell and Campbell (1999) explicitly mentioned that reflection partially consisted of “a desire for self-knowledge” we expected that daily reflection would be positively related to daily search for meaning in life. Such a prediction is also consistent with the results of Steger et al. (2008).

Section snippets

Participants and procedure

Participants were 130 (Mage = 18.66, SD = 0.99, 63.8% female) undergraduate students who received course credit for their participation. For 14 consecutive days, participants were asked to complete a survey at the end of the day before going to sleep. Emails were sent to participants periodically throughout the study to remind them to complete surveys at the end of each day.

Daily reports were included in the final analyses if they were completed between 9 pm and noon of the following day. Entries

Reliability

The data were conceptualized as a multilevel data structure (days nested within persons), and we analyzed the data using HLM 7.0 (Raudenbush, Bryk, & Congdon, 2011). Before conducting the primary analyses, we examined the reliability of each of the daily measures. As recommended by Nezlek (2016), reliabilities were estimated using three level models (items nested within days, days nested within persons).

The reliabilities of the original scales measuring reflection, PA, NA, and ND were not as

Discussion

The present results suggest that self-attentive thinking, particularly in the form of rumination, is associated with decreased well-being, defined in both affective and non-affective terms. These results are largely consistent with, and expand, previous research on relationships between rumination and well-being (e.g., Steger et al., 2008). As expected, relationships between reflection and well-being were not as consistent as relationships between well-being and rumination; however, the

References (33)

  • K.S. Dickson et al.

    Rumination, worry, cognitive avoidance, and behavioral avoidance: Examination of temporal effects

    Behavior Therapy

    (2012)
  • T.B. Kashdan et al.

    When is rumination an adaptive mood repair strategy? Day-to-day rhythms of life in combat veterans with and without posttraumatic stress disorder

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2012)
  • G. Affleck et al.

    Multilevel daily process designs for consulting and clinical psychology: A preface for the perplexed

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1999)
  • E.E. Erikson

    Identity and the life cycle

    Psychological Issues

    (1958)
  • L. Feldman Barrett et al.

    Independence and bipolarity in the structure of current affect

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • V. Frankl

    Man's search for meaning: An introduction to logetheraphy

    (1963)
  • J.J. Genet et al.

    Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood: Results from a diary study

    Emotion

    (2012)
  • S.J. Heine et al.

    The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations

    Personality and Social Psychology Review

    (2006)
  • P.E. Jose et al.

    Rumination as a mediator and moderator of the relationship between unpleasant events and unhappy mood: A daily diary study

    Acta Psychopathologica

    (2015)
  • T.B. Kashdan et al.

    Whether, when, and how is spirituality related to well-being? Moving beyond single occasion questionnaires to understanding daily process

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2012)
  • C.J.M. Maas et al.

    Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling

    Methodology

    (2005)
  • F. Martela et al.

    The three meanings of meaning in life: Distinguishing coherence, purpose, and significance

    The Journal of Positive Psychology

    (2016)
  • A.W. Meade et al.

    Identifying careless responses in survey data

    Psychological Methods

    (2012)
  • N.J. Moberly et al.

    Ruminative self-focus and negative affect: An experience sampling study

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2008)
  • D.B. Newman

    Rumination and Reflection PAID

    (2017, June 29)
  • J.B. Nezlek

    Day-to-day relationships between self-awareness, daily events, and anxiety

    Journal of Personality

    (2002)
  • Cited by (49)

    • Development and psychometrics of a new Emotion-focused Regulation Questionnaire in Chinese

      2022, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      The adaptive or maladaptive functional dichotomy of strategies may promote the integration of strategy tendencies (e.g., Cracco et al., 2015; Naragon-Gainey et al., 2017). Since reflection and reappraisal are both adaptive (e.g., Kim & Kang, 2022; Newman & Nezlek, 2019; Schäfer et al., 2017), they were integrated as a single construct. This is also consistent with the previous finding that reflection tendency could significantly predict reappraisal frequency (Haga, Kraft, & Corby, 2007).

    • Lonely meaning–seekers: The moderating role of search for meaning in the relationship between loneliness and presence of meaning

      2022, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Both these variables are associated with better coping in difficult situations (Silvia & Kashdan, 2009; Korkmaz & Güloğlu, 2021); what is more, they buffer the negative effects of social deprivation (Kawamoto et al., 2017; Smith et al., 2020). Additionally, search for meaning is associated with cognitive interest in various aspects of the self (i.e., reflection; Newman & Nezlek, 2019), which, as shown by the first author's recent study, also weakens the relationship between loneliness and presence of meaning (Borawski, 2022). Other studies among Polish samples revealed an association of search for meaning with various forms of intrinsic religiosity (Kossakowska et al., 2013; Krok, 2015), which is of significance because religiosity (particularly its internalized manifestations) weakens the negative relationship between social disconnectedness and meaning in life (Chan et al., 2019; Reynolds et al., 2020).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text