Gender differences in rumination: A meta-analysis

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.03.019Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Meta-analysis of gender differences in rumination (k = 59; N = 14,321) conducted.

  • Women are more likely to ruminate than are men (Cohen’s d = .24).

  • Women brood (d = .19) and reflect (d = .17) more so than men.

  • We found no evidence of between study heterogeneity or publication bias.

  • Effect sizes of gender were significant and robust, but small in magnitude.

Abstract

Starting in adolescence and continuing through adulthood, women are twice as likely as men to experience depression. According to the response styles theory (RST), gender differences in depression result, in part, from women’s tendency to ruminate more than men. A meta-analysis was performed to evaluate gender differences in rumination in adults (k = 59; N = 14,321); additionally, an analysis of subtypes of rumination – brooding and reflection – was conducted (k = 23). Fixed effects analyses indicated that women scored higher than men in rumination (d = .24, p < .01, SEd = .02), brooding (d = .19, p < .01, SEd = .03) and reflection (d = .17, p < .01, SEd = .03); there was no evidence of heterogeneity or publication bias across studies for these effect sizes. Although statistically significant, the effect sizes for gender differences in rumination were small in magnitude. Results are discussed with respect to the RST and gender differences in depression.

Introduction

Gender differences in rates of depression are well documented in the literature; beginning in adolescence, rates of depression in women are greater than in men, and by adulthood, women are twice as likely as men to become depressed (for a review, see Kessler, 2006). Numerous theories have been set forth to explain the gender differences in depression, citing psychological, sociocultural, and biological factors (for reviews, see Boughten and Street, 2007, Hankin and Abramson, 2001, Hyde et al., 2008). A prominent psychological theory, the response styles theory (RST; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987, Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991), purports that women have a greater tendency to ruminate on their depressive symptoms and distress than do men, and this contributes to greater rates of depression in women. According to the RST, rumination involves repetitively and passively focusing on symptoms of distress and on the possible causes and consequences of these symptoms. Because rumination enhances the effects of depressed mood on thinking, impairs effective problem solving, interferes with instrumental behavior, and erodes social support, the initial symptoms of depression among people who chronically ruminate are likely to become more severe and evolve into episodes of major depression and rumination may prolong current depressive episodes (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008). The RST has spurred a large body of empirical research on the role of rumination in the onset and duration of depression (for reviews, see Nolen-Hoeksema, 2012, Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Furthermore, the theoretical assumptions of the RST have been incorporated into broader models of depression risk (e.g., Hankin and Abramson, 2001, Hyde et al., 2008).

Although evidence for gender differences in rumination has emerged consistently in the literature, the magnitude of this difference has varied from study to study. In an effort to systematically review the rumination literature in children and adolescents, Rood and colleagues (2009) conducted a meta-analysis of gender differences in rumination in youth. Results of their analysis indicated that gender differences in rumination are quite small in children (d = .14) with girls significantly more likely to ruminate than boys. In adolescence, this gender difference was significant and larger in magnitude (d = .36). The authors reported that these results provide some evidence of the RST in youth and suggest that during adolescence, the increased magnitude of gender differences in rumination may parallel that seen in rates of depression.

Another meta-analysis, conducted by Tamres and colleagues (2002), examined gender differences in coping mechanisms and included 10 studies reporting on gender differences in rumination. Results yielded a small but significant effect of gender on rumination (d = .19), with women more likely to ruminate than men. However, the Tamres analysis differs from the current study in several important ways. First, Tamres and colleagues focused on methods of coping (e.g., their search terms included “coping”, “stress management”) in response to a broad range of stressors, including relationship stress, physical pain, and health issues (e.g., cancer, artificial insemination). Thus, their analysis was based on a much broader definition of rumination (i.e., including thoughts about physical pain, academic performance, relationships) rather than specifically on depressive rumination as defined above. Secondly, their review included studies of children and adults and results were collapsed across these groups, thus obscuring potential influences of developmental stages on the gender differences in rumination (Rood et al., 2009). In contrast, the current study focuses specifically on depressive rumination in adults so as to directly address the tenets of the RST.

Despite the prominence of the RST in the field, no quantitative analysis of gender differences in depressive rumination has been conducted in the adult literature. The current review was conducted to provide a meta-analysis of the literature on gender differences in rumination in adult samples. We reviewed studies that included a measure of rumination, focusing on depressive rumination, which is most commonly measured by the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS; Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991) and the Rumination on Sadness Scale (RSS; Conway, Csank, Holm, & Blake, 2000), both of which are self-report measures. The RRS is a 22-item subscale from the Response Styles Questionnaire; participants rate how often they experience each response when they feel sad, blue, or depressed. Items focus on the meaning of rumination, on the subjective feelings related to depressed mood, on symptoms, and on consequences and causes of the mood (Luminet, 2004). Sample items on the RRS include, “Think, ‘What am I doing to deserve this?’” and “Go away by yourself and think about why you feel this way.” The RSS is a 13-item measure assessing how often participants experience each response in situations in which they feel sad, down, or blue. Items focus on the intensity and repetitive quality of ruminative thoughts, the difficulty with stopping ruminative thoughts, attempts at understanding the nature of one’s distress, and the lack of instrumental goal orientation (Luminet, 2004). Sample items on the RSS include “I repeatedly analyze and keep thinking about the reasons for my sadness” and “I exhaust myself by thinking so much about myself and the reasons for my sadness.” A review of the psychometric properties of the RRS and RSS can be found in Luminet (2004).

In addition to our analysis of global measures of depressive rumination, we examined gender differences in subtypes or components of rumination – brooding and reflection – obtained from a factor analysis of the RRS items that remained after eliminating items that could be argued to overlap with depressive symptoms (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). This scale consists of five items assessing brooding, or passive, perseverative, maladaptive self-focused thought, and five items assessing reflection, a neutrally valenced, less maladaptive self-reflective strategy. Compared to reflection, brooding may be more strongly associated with depression severity, particularly over time, and some evidence suggests that gender differences in brooding may be greater than gender differences in reflection (e.g., Treynor et al., 2003). By estimating the overall effect size of gender differences in rumination, as well as the brooding and reflection components of rumination, we sought to provide the field with a quantitative analysis of past research and examine the basic tenets of the RST regarding gender differences in rumination in adults. Furthermore, the results of the current study may serve to guide future research efforts in elucidating the gender differences in rates of depression.

Section snippets

Literature search and inclusion criteria

We followed two steps to ensure we reviewed all published papers reporting on gender differences in rumination. First, using the citation index in Web of Science, we reviewed all studies through October 2011 citing the original publication of the RRS (Nolen-Hoeksema & Morrow, 1991) or the RSS (Conway et al., 2000), as they are the most commonly used self-report measures of individual differences in the tendency to engage in depressive rumination. Next, we searched the PsycINFO and Web of

Rumination analysis

Our review of the literature on gender differences in rumination yielded 57 studies, including 59 separate samples, which met our inclusion criteria for analysis. The majority of studies included in the analysis used the RRS or the RSS to assess depressive rumination, however a few studies used other measures [Repetitive Negative Thinking subscale of the Repetitive Thought Questionnaire (RTQ; McEvoy, Mahoney, & Moulds, 2010); the Rumination subscale of the Rumination-Reflection Scale (RRQ;

Discussion

The current study provides a quantitative analysis of the literature on gender differences in depressive rumination and an examination of the basic tenets of the RST in adults. In our review of the literature, we identified studies that reported on gender differences in rumination as well as requested data from studies that did not report on gender differences, calculated independent effect sizes for each sample, and summarized these findings into an aggregate effect size for gender in the

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the researchers who provided us with unpublished data to include in the meta-analysis. We would also like to thank our team of research assistants who provided assistance with the extensive literature review. This research was supported in part by grants from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression and the National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (HD007289).

References (25)

  • P.M. McEvoy et al.

    Are worry, rumination, and post-event processing one and the same? Development of the repetitive thinking questionnaire

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2010)
  • L. Rood et al.

    The influence of emotion-focused rumination and distraction on depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth: A meta-analytic review

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2009)
  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    The Temple-Wisconsin Cognitive Vulnerability to Depression Project: Lifetime history of axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2000)
  • M.F. Armey et al.

    Brooding and pondering: Isolating the active ingredients of depressive rumination with exploratory factor analysis and structural equation modeling

    Assessment

    (2009)
  • S. Baldwin et al.

    A primer on meta-analysis in clinical psychology

    Journal of Experimental Psychopathology

    (2011)
  • S. Boughten et al.

    Integrated review of the social and psychological gender differences in depression

    Australian Psychologist

    (2007)
  • M.J. Cambron et al.

    Explaining gender differences in depression: An interpersonal contingent self-esteem perspective

    Sex Roles

    (2009)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (Version 2.2.064) [Computer Software]. Englewood, NJ:...
  • S. Duval et al.

    Trim and fill: A simple funnel plot based method for testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis

    Biometrics

    (2000)
  • B.L. Hankin

    Development of sex differences in depressive and co-occurring anxious symptoms during adolescence: Descriptive trajectories and potential explanations in a multiwave prospective study

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2009)
  • B.L. Hankin et al.

    Development of gender differences in depression: An elaborated cognitive vulnerability-transactional stress theory

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2001)
  • Cited by (390)

    • Daily worry, rumination, and sleep in late life

      2024, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text