Seeing or smelling? Assessing personality on the basis of different stimuli

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.026Get rights and content

Highlights

  • I examine personality ratings based on photos, body odor and their combination.

  • Neuroticism and dominance were accurately assessed based on body odor.

  • Extraversion and neuroticism were accurately rated from combined photos and odor.

  • Scent attractiveness correlated with targets’ personalities.

Abstract

This study examines whether people can accurately assess personality on the basis of facial images and body odor and whether attractiveness influences these relationships. Three personality dimensions of target individuals – neuroticism, extraversion and dominance – were measured with the NEO Five-Factor Inventory, a one-item measure of dominance and the reports of close acquaintances. Naive observers assessed neuroticism and dominance at above-chance levels based on body odor, and they assessed extraversion (and in some cases, neuroticism) at above-chance levels based on either facial images alone or body odor and facial images presented together. The accuracy differed depending on the sex of the targets and the raters. In addition, facial and body odor attractiveness predicted the targets’ personalities and the assessments of their personalities. These results show that the accuracy of personality assessment changes when judges assess different types of stimuli.

Introduction

Research on the judgment of strangers in zero-acquaintance situations suggests that people are able to quite accurately assess the personality traits of others on the basis of videotaped behavior or short interactions (e.g., Borkenau and Liebler, 1992, Carney et al., 2007, Watson, 1989). Other studies examining the accuracy of personality judgments based on photographs alone (for a review, see Zebrowitz, 1997) suggested relatively high accuracy in assessments of extraversion (e.g., Naumann et al., 2009, Penton-Voak et al., 2006) and in some studies, in assessments of neuroticism and openness to experience (Naumann et al., 2009, Penton-Voak et al., 2006 found this result for male faces only). However, little research has investigated sources of information over and above appearance. In one of a few existing studies, Borkenau and Liebler (1992) showed that additional sources of information might in some cases increase the accuracy of personality judgments.

Borkenau and Liebler’s (1992) results are consistent with the Realistic Accuracy Model (RAM) created by Funder (1995). The author of this model suggests that judgmental accuracy is a function of the availability, detection, and utilization of relevant cues, and that variables such as the characteristics of the judges, or the traits being judged might affect this accuracy (Funder, 1995). It has yet to be determined exactly what makes accurate judgments more likely, but some studies suggest that accuracy should increase as the amount of available information or the amount of certain types of information increases (Carney et al., 2007).

It could be presumed that the availability of olfactory cues might also influence the accuracy of personality judgments. In humans, axillary odor is thought to perform a signaling function because of high activity of the glands reacting to changes in our physiology or psychological states (Sato, 1977). In addition, axillary hair, which might serve as a trap for signaling chemicals, contributes to this function. Recently, Sorokowska, Sorokowski, and Szmajke (2012) showed that human body odor might convey information about personality traits. In their study, judges’ ratings, which were based on body odor alone, were relatively congruent with the targets’ self-assessed extraversion, neuroticism and dominance. The authors suggested that such assessments were possible because personality and body odor are influenced with the same biological mechanisms (Carver and Miller, 2006, Zuckerman, 1995). In addition, emotional experiences (which affect body odor, as demonstrated by, for example, Ackerl et al., 2002, Chen and Haviland-Jones, 2000) characteristic of some personality profiles might influence body odor to such an extent that it can be smelled by others. Moreover, Havlicek, Roberts, and Flegr (2005) and Rantala, Eriksson, Vainikka, and Kortet (2006) showed that individual dominance is related to the attractiveness of one’s scent. Thus, it seems likely that initial impressions regarding the personalities of others are influenced by the information inferred from their body odor and/or its attractiveness.

The aforementioned studies suggest that it could be possible to assess some traits of personality on the basis of facial pictures or body odor. In this study I analyzed the accuracy of assessments of personality on the basis of facial pictures alone, body odor alone, and facial pictures and body odor together. In addition, I analyzed the possible effect of attractiveness. In the previous study regarding body odor (Sorokowska et al., 2012), the only traits judged accurately were neuroticism, extraversion and dominance; thus, the judges in the present study rated only these dimensions. Because males and females differ in body odor intensity (Chen & Haviland-Jones, 1999) and olfactory abilities (Doty & Cameron, 2009), I performed the analyses separately for men and women (as targets and raters).

Section snippets

Materials and methods

The study examined personality assessments based on the target’s (a) body odor, (b) facial picture and (c) body odor and facial picture. The two best, most commonly used methods to measure personality are self-report and informant reports. Both contain large components of accuracy (e.g., Funder, 1995). Thus, both self-reported data and knowledgeable informants’ reports about targets’ traits were included as criteria to assess the accuracy of the personality judgments made on the basis of the

Results

The internal consistencies (alphas) of the NEO-FFI scales were .80 for neuroticism and .77 for extraversion. The knowledgeable informants’ ratings significantly correlated with the targets’ self-assessments: rs(48) = .44 for neuroticism, rs(48) = .50 for extraversion and rs(48) = .52 for dominance (all ps < .05).

Discussion

The results of my study suggest that the assessments of certain personality traits change when strangers assess different types of stimuli. When strangers based their ratings on body odor, their assessments agreed with the targets’ self-assessments of neuroticism and dominance. As for a facial picture, the strangers’ ratings agreed with self-reported and informant-reported extraversion and informant-reported neuroticism. For body odor and a facial picture presented together, the strangers’

Conclusions

The findings presented here show that the assessments of certain personality traits change when strangers assess different types of stimuli. Neuroticism and dominance were assessed at above-chance levels based on body odor, and extraversion (and in some cases, neuroticism) was assessed at above-chance levels based on either facial images alone or the combination of body odor and facial images. The results suggest that ratings of personality and attractiveness could be highly dependent on the

References (23)

  • J.D. Foster

    Beauty is mostly in the eye of the beholder: Olfactory versus visual cues of attractiveness

    The Journal of Social Psychology

    (2008)
  • Cited by (23)

    • Body-odor based assessments of sex and personality – Non-significant differences between blind and sighted odor raters

      2019, Physiology and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Further, based on body odors, people are able to relatively accurately assess biological qualities such as sex [21,40]. Finally, body odors were shown to be a relatively accurate indicators of personality, namely neuroticism and dominance [42,44,48], with dominance [17,38,42] and neuroticism [42] additionally affecting perception of body odor attractiveness. Despite the undeniable role body odors play in human interactions, people exhibit different sensitivity to body odors in the social context.

    • The impact of artificial fragrances on the assessment of mate quality cues in body odor

      2016, Evolution and Human Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      In support of this, research suggests that humans indeed use olfactory cues present in odor to assess a range of qualities. For example, humans can assess an individual's sex (Schleidt et al., 1981), personality (Sorokowska, 2013), diet (Fialová, Roberts, & Havlíček, 2013), genetic compatibility (Havlicek & Roberts, 2009; Havlíček & Roberts, 2013) and health status (Moshkin et al., 2012) via odor. Humans also have the capacity to recognize kin via body odor (Ferdenzi, Schaal, & Roberts, 2010; Roberts et al., 2005; Weisfeld, Czilli, Phillips, Gall, & Lichtman, 2003), which is important in sexual selection in order to avoid inbreeding.

    • Determinants of human olfactory performance: A cross-cultural study

      2015, Science of the Total Environment
      Citation Excerpt :

      Nevertheless, one should not underestimate the importance of the sense of smell (for a review see: Shepherd, 2004). Olfaction has many important functions which can be divided into three groups (Stevenson, 2010), related to (1) ingestion (Cannon et al., 1983; Capaldi and Privitera, 2007; Fallon and Rozin, 1983; Porter et al., 2007), (2) avoidance of environmental dangers (Cain and Turk, 1985; Curtis and Biran, 2001; Willander and Larsson, 2007), and (3) social communication (Ackerl et al., 2002; Havlicek et al., 2008; Hold and Schleidt, 1977; Mitro et al., 2012; Sorokowska, 2013; Wedekind et al., 1995). Olfactory sensitivity varies among individuals (Murphy et al., 2003), and culture is likely to significantly influence olfactory preferences and attitudes towards olfaction (Ayabe-Kanamura et al., 1998; Distel et al., 1999; Seo et al., 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text