Perfectionism and social goals: What do perfectionists want to achieve in social situations?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.02.002Get rights and content

Abstract

With a sample of 367 college students, the current study examined how two dimensions of perfectionism (PS: personal standards and COM: concern over mistakes) are related to social achievement goals and social content goals. COM was linked to less desirable types of social goals (e.g., social demonstration-approach, social demonstration-avoid, popularity, and dominance goals) and had null relations with adaptive social goals (e.g., social development, intimacy, and nurturance goals). In contrast, PS was related to adaptive social goals (e.g., social development, nurturance, intimacy, and leadership goals) and had no relation with maladaptive social goals (e.g., dominance goals). Despite all these benefits, PS was also positively related to social demonstration-avoid goals, which have been consistently linked to psychological ill-being and negative social outcomes.

Highlights

► Concern over mistakes relates to social demonstration-approach and -avoid goals. ► Concern over mistakes relates to popularity and dominance goals. ► Personal standards relate to nurturance and intimacy goals. ► Personal standards relate to social development and demonstration-avoid goals. ► All sub types of perfectionism may involve drawbacks in terms of social goals.

Introduction

Perfectionism is a personality disposition, which encompasses the tendency to strive for perfection, intolerance for errors and mistakes, and evaluating oneself in an overly critical manner (Frost et al., 1990, Hewitt and Flett, 1991). Different research programs have used various definitions and measures. However, various scales have shown statistical overlap and have formed two distinct factors if subjected to a factor analysis simultaneously. Self-oriented perfectionism, personal standards, and organization (i.e., perfectionistic strivings) tended to cluster together and socially prescribed perfectionism, concern over mistakes, doubts about actions, parental expectations, and parental criticism (i.e., perfectionistic concerns) tended to form a separate factor (Frost, Heimberg, Holt, Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993). To summarize, perfectionistic strivings are characterized by high standards but do not involve an overly critical evaluation of the self, whereas perfectionistic concerns involve hypersensitivity to externally imposed high standards and criticism (see Stoeber & Otto, 2006 for a review).

Perfectionistic concerns have consistently been linked to maladjustment. However, perfectionistic strivings have not shown such deleterious effects and have often related to desirable outcomes (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). These associations with desirable outcomes have prompted some researchers to argue that perfectionistic strivings may be beneficial, leading to terms such as adaptive, positive, or healthy perfectionism (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). However, not all researchers agree with the notion that a sub-type of perfectionism can be adaptive (Flett & Hewitt, 2006). Thus, we began our paper with an agnostic position regarding this debate but used the terms, perfectionistic strivings vs. perfectionistic concerns, when reviewing studies using different measures of perfectionism for brevity’s sake.

Given that perfectionism involves striving for extremely high standards, perfectionism researchers have often examined goals in relation to perfectionism. The current study directs this line of inquiry into the social domain. Compared to the academic or sport related domain, little attention has been paid to social goals. However, social goals have been acknowledged as important for a wide array of outcomes such as academic adjustment, social adjustment, and psychological wellbeing (Ryan and Shim, 2006, Ryan and Shim, 2008).

The present research extends the current literature on perfectionism in important ways. First, perfectionism research has examined interpersonal relationship outcomes and social anxiety (Flett et al., 2001, Gilman et al., 2011, Ommundsen et al., 2005, Shumaker and Rodebaugh, 2009). But we do not know the motivation underlying such social outcomes. Given that goals, by definition, initiate, energize, and direct behavior (Elliot, 2005), social goals may help explain certain associations between perfectionism and social outcomes. Second, various types of social goals have been examined in relation to psychological well-being and academic and social adjustment (Elliot et al., 2006, Horst et al., 2007, Jarvinen and Nicholls, 1996, Ryan and Shim, 2006, Ryan and Shim, 2008). As a result, the nature and consequences of social goals have been identified. Accordingly, the current investigation is likely to contribute to the current debate on whether some aspects of perfectionism can be adaptive (Flett and Hewitt, 2006, Stoeber and Otto, 2006).

Perfectionistic strivings seem to be related to high investment and making progress toward goals, flexibility in adjusting goals when necessary, and disengaging from unattainable goals and pursuing new alternative goals, and the eventual attainment of goals. In contrast, perfectionistic concerns may hamper such self-regulatory processes related to goal pursuits (Gaudreau and Thompson, 2010, O’Connor and Forgan, 2007, Powers et al., 2011). The evidence is inconclusive in terms of the relationships between perfectionistic strivings and goal related satisfaction (Flett and Hewitt, 2006, Slade and Owens, 1998). Although goal progress has often been examined for self-chosen goals (e.g., losing weight) involving concrete standards (e.g., going to the gym three times a week), some researchers have examined perfectionism in relation to approach vs. avoidance motivational tendencies. Perfectionistic strivings involve approach tendencies, leading to pursuit of goals appetitive in nature (i.e., pursuing positive outcomes), while perfectionistic concerns involve avoidance tendencies, leading to pursuit of goals aversive in nature (i.e., avoiding negative outcomes) (Slade & Owens, 1998). Individuals with high perfectionistic strivings and those with high perfectionistic concerns tend to be driven by a desire for success and a fear of failure, respectively. In support of such a postulation, perfectionistic concerns were linked to the behavioral inhibition system that controls aversive motivation (O’Connor & Forgan, 2007).

Some prior research has adopted the achievement goal theory perspective. Achievement goals can be distinguished according to the orientation to competence (mastery goals focusing on developing competence vs. performance goals focusing on demonstrating competence) and the valence of potential outcomes (approaching positive outcomes vs. avoiding negative outcomes). Among these resulting four different types of achievement goals (mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance, performance-approach, performance-avoidance goals; Elliot & McGregor, 2001), a fear of failure predicts all goals except mastery-approach goals while a desire for success is related to two approach (mastery-approach and performance-approach) goals. In prior studies, perfectionistic strivings predicted mastery-approach and performance-approach goals while perfectionistic concerns were related to performance-avoidance goals (Speirs Neumeister & Finch, 2006). However, an association between perfectionistic concerns and performance-approach goals (Verner-Filion & Gaudreau, 2010) and similar levels of mastery-approach goals for individuals characterized by high perfectionistic strivings vs. high perfectionistic concerns (Hanchon, 2010) have been reported. Taken together, in the academic domain, perfectionistic concerns tend to be associated with less adaptive motivational profiles as compared to perfectionistic strivings.

To examine the relationships between perfectionism and social motivation, we adopted two approaches to social goals. First, the social content goal approach examines specific outcomes that students like to achieve from social relations. We examined five content goals drawn from Jarvinen and Nicholls (1996): nurturance, intimacy, leadership, popularity, and dominance. In general, intimacy and nurturance goals were linked to social satisfaction and peer ratings of popularity (Jarvinen & Nicholls, 1996). Popularity goals were related to social satisfaction and peer ratings of popularity but also linked to negative attitudes about school and disengagement (Ryan, Hicks, & Midgley, 1997). Dominance goals were mostly related to low social satisfaction and low peer ratings of popularity (Jarvinen & Nicholls, 1996) as well as maladaptive forms of engagement and low achievement (Cillessen and Mayeux, 2004, Kiefer and Ryan, 2008). Even though both dominance and leadership goals emanate from a power motive, leadership goals are a more positive manifestation. Leadership goals were not examined in prior research but may yield positive outcomes related to leadership qualities such as prosocial behavior.

The second approach involves the achievement goal theory perspective applied to the social domain. While social content goals represent the outcomes that students want to achieve in social contexts, social achievement goals represent an overarching purpose for engagement. Three types of goals mirroring academic achievement goals (mastery goals and performance-approach and -avoidance goals) have been proposed (Ryan and Shim, 2006, Ryan and Shim, 2008): Social development goals involve reasons for “developing” social competence such as improving social relationships and social skills; social demonstration-approach goals represent the goals of “demonstrating” superior social competence, typically by garnering positive feedback from others and gaining social prestige; and social demonstration-avoid goals represent the goals of “concealing” inferior social competence, typically by avoiding negative judgments from others (Ryan & Shim, 2006).

Research has shown that social development goals related to many positive outcomes while social demonstration-avoid goals consistently related to maladjustment (e.g., perceptions of social relationships, loneliness, social worry, fear of negative evaluations, depression when faced with interpersonal stress, and anxious solitary behavior). Social demonstration-approach goals related to heightened social-efficacy, peer ratings of popularity, but also positively related to aggression (Elliot et al., 2006, Horst et al., 2007, Ryan and Shim, 2006, Ryan and Shim, 2008).

Given that the current study is the first empirical investigation, there is no available data for hypotheses regarding the relationships between perfectionism and social goals. However, individuals with perfectionistic strivings may also strive for excellence in their social relationships (Flett et al., 2001), leading to the adoption of social development and social demonstration-approach goals. In contrast, given the sensitivity to others’ evaluations inherent in perfectionistic concerns, we expected that perfectionistic concerns would be related to high social demonstration-approach and avoid goals (Slade and Owens, 1998, Verner-Filion and Gaudreau, 2010). Perfectionistic concerns have been associated with undesirable social behaviors (Flett et al., 2001, Gilman et al., 2011, Ommundsen et al., 2005). Thus, we expected that perfectionistic strivings would be related to more positive social content goals such as intimacy, nurturance, and leadership goals while perfectionistic concerns would be related to less desirable social content goals such as popularity and dominance goals.

Section snippets

Participants

Three hundred and sixty seven college students in a university in the Midwestern region of the United States participated for course credit. The sample was predominantly European American (95%). The mean age of participants was 21.56 years old and 78.5% were female. They were freshman (32%), junior (13%), sophomore (30%), and senior (25%) students enrolled in various Educational Psychology courses.

Measures

A 7-point Likert-type scale was used for all measures ranging from strongly disagree (1) to

Results

The analyses were conducted by utilizing both a variable-centered (i.e., multiple regression) and a person-centered approach (i.e., cluster analysis). In general, perfectionism researchers have utilized one of these two approaches in previous studies but rarely both (Stoeber & Otto, 2006). A variable-centered approach is effective in estimating the unique contribution of one variable while controlling for the levels of other variables. However, in reality, individuals often show combinations of

Discussion

The current research represents the first investigation of perfectionism in relation to social goals and contributes to our understanding of how individuals with perfectionism approach their social relationships. In the regression analyses, PS was related to desirable goals such as social development, nurturance, intimacy, and leadership goals but not associated with less desirable goals such as social demonstration-approach and dominance goals. A significant PS by COM interaction indicated

References (31)

  • A.J. Elliot et al.

    Approach and avoidance motivation in the social domain

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2006)
  • A.J. Elliot et al.

    A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework

    Journal Of Personality & Social Psychology

    (2001)
  • G.L. Flett et al.

    Positive vs. negative perfectionism in psychopathology: A comment on Slade and Owens’s dual process model

    Behavior Modification

    (2006)
  • G.L. Flett et al.

    Perfectionism, beliefs, and adjustment in dating relationships

    Current Psychology

    (2001)
  • R.O. Frost et al.

    The dimensions of perfectionism

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (1990)
  • Cited by (25)

    • How do adolescents approach social relationships?: The cost of perfectionistic concerns

      2019, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Maladaptive reasons for seeking social relationships (e.g., avoiding being embarrassed) and/or expectations about social relationships (e.g., dominance over others) may lead to interpersonal conflict (Jarvinen & Nicholls, 1996; Ryan & Shim, 2006, 2008). Perfectionistic concerns were related to less optimal types of social goals such as avoiding being judged and dominating others, whereas perfectionistic strivings were related to desirable forms of social goals such as improving one's social skills and supporting others (Shim & Fletcher, 2012). Understanding social motivations for peer relationships and how perfectionism might be associated with social motivations suggests a potential mechanism underlying perfectionists' experience of social disconnection.

    • An investigation of the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism with burnout, engagement, self-regulation, and academic achievement

      2017, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus far, several studies have tested the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism in the academic setting. Research on the 2 × 2 model with university students has mostly allocated attention to the associations between subtypes of perfectionism and various processes and indicators of psychological adjustment, such as academic achievement goals (Speirs Neumeister, Fletcher, & Burney, 2015), social achievement goals and social content goals (Shim & Fletcher, 2012), self-determination (Gaudreau, 2015; Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010; Gaudreau et al., 2016), academic and life satisfaction (Franche, Gaudreau, & Miranda, 2012; Gaudreau, 2015; Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010; Gaudreau et al., 2016), depression (Douilliez & Lefèvre, 2011), stress (Franche & Gaudreau, 2016), test anxiety and pre-exam coping (Arana & Furlan, 2016), positive and negative affect (Franche & Gaudreau, 2016; Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010), joy (Gaudreau, 2015), vitality (Franche & Gaudreau, 2016), and self-esteem (Taylor, Papay, Webb, & Reeve, 2016). Some studies have also examined the relations between subtypes of perfectionism and academic achievement with subjective indicators of achievement, such as academic goal progress (Franche & Gaudreau, 2016; Gaudreau, 2015; Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010) and self-reported GPA (Franche et al., 2012; Gaudreau, 2012).

    • When perfectionism is coupled with low achievement: The effects on academic engagement and help seeking in middle school

      2016, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Then, we entered the interaction between perfectionism and math grades (PS X Math Grade and COM X Math Grade) at the third step. Although not our central aim, the PS X COM interaction term was tested given previous studies documenting that the effects of PS and COM depend on the level of the other (Gaudreau & Thompson, 2010; Shim & Fletcher, 2012). We kept gender, math achievement, PS, and COM in the regression model regardless of their significance to estimate the main effects of the variables after controlling for the other variables.

    • Perception matters for clinical perfectionism and social anxiety

      2015, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, low levels of clinical perfectionism (low levels of both maladaptive perfectionism and personal standards) appears to be as problematic as high levels, which is not consistent with the theory that clinical perfectionism produces a notable exacerbation of symptoms, or increased vulnerability to symptoms. Notably, the findings regarding social interaction anxiety are consistent with results from Shim and Fletcher (2012), who found that concern over mistakes was more strongly related to social approach goals (i.e., demonstrating social desirability: such as being seen as having lots of friends) when standards were low. It seems plausible that social approach goals would be related to social interaction anxiety because these goals both involve interactions with others (versus specific fears of scrutiny during performance settings).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text