Positive traits linked to less pain through lower pain catastrophizing

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.10.040Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study examined the association between positive traits, pain catastrophizing, and pain perceptions. We hypothesized that pain catastrophizing would mediate the relationship between positive traits and pain. First, participants (n = 114) completed the Trait Hope Scale, the Life Orientation Test-Revised, and the Pain Catastrophizing Scale. Participants then completed the experimental pain stimulus, a cold pressor task, by submerging their hand in a circulating water bath (0 °C) for as long as tolerable. Immediately following the task, participants completed the Short-Form McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ-SF). Pearson correlation found associations between hope and pain catastrophizing (r = −.41, p < .01) and MPQ-SF scores (r = −.20, p < .05). Optimism was significantly associated with pain catastrophizing (r = −.44, p < .01) and MPQ-SF scores (r = −.19, p < .05). Bootstrapping, a non-parametric resampling procedure, tested for mediation and supported our hypothesis that pain catastrophizing mediated the relationship between positive traits and MPQ-SF pain report. To our knowledge, this investigation is the first to establish that the protective link between positive traits and experimental pain operates through lower pain catastrophizing.

Highlights

► We examine positive traits, pain catastrophizing and pain report. ► Participants completed an experimental pain stimulus, a cold pressor task. ► Associations found between positive traits, pain catastrophizing and pain report. ► Pain catastrophizing mediated the relationship between positive traits and pain.

Introduction

Pain affects more than 76 million people in the United States, or about 26% of the population. Annual estimates of healthcare costs for those suffering with chronic pain exceed $6000 per person (Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, 2006). Moreover, chronic pain is a continual burden affecting both physical and mental functioning. Negative consequences of chronic pain can include long hours of rehabilitation, loss of work and income, and hospitalization (Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, 2006).

Pain is a complex phenomenon influenced by biological, psychological, and social factors. The control gate theory of pain is a popular psychological explanation that suggests that perception and interpretation are important in understanding pain (Siegele, 1974). When individuals assess pain, their characteristic ways of thinking (i.e., cognitive sets), and their feelings are significant determinants of their subsequent pain response. For instance, anxiety can intensify the experience of pain, whereas, positive affect can attenuate pain (Siegele, 1974). The present study focused on the psychological experience of pain and addressed the influence of positive psychological traits and negative cognitive sets; specifically, how hope, optimism and pain catastrophizing affected pain outcomes.

Pain catastrophizing is an exaggerated negative response to actual or anticipated pain. It is the tendency to ruminate, magnify, or feel helplessness about pain experience (Sullivan, Bishop, & Pivik, 1995). Previous studies have found that pain catastrophizers report more pain in clinical and experimental settings (France et al., 2002, Rhudy et al., 2009). Further, pain catastrophizers generally report significantly more pain-related thoughts, and experience more emotional distress, and greater pain intensity than non-catastrophizers (Moldovan, Onac, Vantu, Szentagotai, & Onac, 2009).

Negative personality traits, or response patterns and their association with pain have historically been the primary focus of clinical research (Pearce & Porter, 1983). Neuroticism, a negative psychological trait that can produce nervousness, moodiness, and sensitivity to negative stimuli, has been shown to influence pain report (Charles, Gatz, Kato, & Pedersen, 2008), and higher neuroticism can provoke more negative affect in emotion-inducing situations (Larsen & Ketelaar, 1989). Other recent research suggests that pain catastrophizing mediates the relationship between self-handicapping and pain. Self-handicapping is a defense mechanism whereby individuals generate hurdles prior to a performance that affects their attributions after the performance. Individuals who self-handicap tend to pain catastrophize and report higher pain (Uysal & Lu, 2010).

Within the past decade, there has been a shift from studying negative traits and emotions to also examining the important role of positive traits and emotions (Fredrickson, 2001, Lyubomirsky et al., 2005, Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Emerging evidence suggests that levels of hope, a positive psychological construct, and optimism, described as a generalized expectancy of positive outcomes are linked with lower pain reports (Berg et al., 2008, Geers et al., 2008, Snyder et al., 2005). In addition, positive emotions and psychological resilience are associated with less pain catastrophizing (Ong, Zautra, & Reid, 2010).

Optimism and hope are positive traits that both have links to adaptive outcomes (Scheier and Carver, 1985, Scheier and Carver, 1987). Despite their similarities, the two constructs differ in important ways. Optimism is a dispositional trait conceived as an explanatory style whereby individuals focus their cognitions on distancing themselves from negative outcomes (Scheier & Carver, 1985). Conversely, hope is a construct conceived as dispositional (Snyder et al., 1991), state (Snyder et al., 1996), or goal-specific (Feldman, Rand, & Kahle-Wrobleski, 2009). Importantly, compared to optimism, hope has been shown to better describe an individual’s emphasis on positive, goal-directed cognitions (Snyder, 2002).

Previous research suggests that hope and optimism are strongly associated but not identical, with correlations ranging from .51 to .55 (Magaletta and Oliver, 1999, Rand, 2009). In addition, statistical modeling has differentiated hope and optimism as distinct but related constructs. Bryant and Cvengros (2004) found that when analyzing hope and optimism, a goodness-of-fit model with separate second-order factors had greater explanatory power than a model analyzing the constructs as a single global factor. Moreover, hope and optimism had divergent patterns of association with coping and self-efficacy. They suggest that researchers interested in future physical and emotional outcomes should examine the constructs separately.

There is a need to examine the relationships among positive psychological traits, pain, and negative reactions to pain (i.e., pain catastrophizing). Therefore, in the present study, we aimed to describe the mechanism through which positive traits and pain perception are linked. We tested the hypothesis that pain catastrophizing mediates the relationship between the positive traits, hope and optimism, and pain perception.

Section snippets

Participants

An initial telephone screening of 274 participants deemed 96 participants medically ineligible. As we obtained data for this study as part of larger study (Pulvers & Hood, 2010) for which smoking status was an important variable, 32 participants were ineligible due to an unstable smoking rate (any smoking rate was acceptable, so long as participants were smoking at their current rate for at least 1 year). Results from the larger study indicated that gender and smoking status were associated with

Correlations

We hypothesized that lower hope and optimism would be associated with higher pain catastrophizing and higher pain. The correlation between hope and optimism (r = .61, p < .01) was similar to previous research studies that found the constructs to be related yet distinct. Hope and pain catastrophizing were significantly associated (r = −.41, p < .01) and hope and total scores on the MPQ-SF were significantly associated (r = −.20, p < .05). In addition, optimism and pain catastrophizing were significantly

Discussion

The present study investigated whether pain catastrophizing mediated the relationship between hope and optimism and pain perception. Our findings indicated that individuals low in hope or optimism reported higher pain catastrophizing and had higher pain reports. These results are consistent with the literature that pain catastrophizing often mediates pain reports (Ong et al., 2010, Uysal and Lu, 2010), and that lower levels of hope and/or optimism result in higher levels of pain perception (

Acknowledgments

This research was funded in part by NIGMS MARC Grant GM-08807. The authors would like to thank Sara Bayles, Jessica Edwards, Sara Margetta, Anna Meldau, Ines Pandzic, Ashleigh Shepard, Mae Talicuran, and Laura Thode for their help with data collection and management.

References (41)

  • S. Charles et al.

    Physical health 25 years later: The predictive ability of neuroticism

    Health Psychology

    (2008)
  • S.J. Coen et al.

    Neuroticism Influences Brain Activity During the Experience of Visceral Pain

    Gastroenterology

    (2011)
  • L.A. Curry et al.

    Role of hope in academic and sport achievement

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • J. Eliott et al.

    The discursive properties of ‘hope’: A qualitative analysis of cancer patients’ speech

    Qualitative Health Research

    (2002)
  • D.B. Feldman et al.

    Hope and goal attainment: Testing a basic prediction of hope theory

    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (2009)
  • B.L. Fredrickson

    The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions

    American Psychologist

    (2001)
  • A.L. Geers et al.

    Dispositional optimism and thoughts of well-being determine sensitivity to an experimental pain task

    Annals of Behavioral Medicine

    (2008)
  • L.D. Jamner et al.

    Pain inhibition, nicotine, and gender

    Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology

    (1998)
  • R.B. Kanarek et al.

    Sucrose consumption enhances the analgesic effects of cigarette smoking in male and female smokers Sucrose, nicotine and pain sensitivity

    Psychopharmacology

    (2004)
  • I.E. Lamé et al.

    Test–retest stability of the Pain Catastrophizing Scale and the Tampa Scale for Kinesiophobia in chronic pain patients over a longer period of time

    Journal of Health Psychology

    (2008)
  • Cited by (64)

    • Hope

      2023, Encyclopedia of Mental Health, Third Edition: Volume 1-3
    • The science of hope

      2020, The Lancet Oncology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, interventions based on Hope Theory have been shown to reduce perceptions of pain in patients with chronic pain.36 This connection between hope and pain perception is likely to be mediated by so-called pain catastrophisation, with research showing that higher-hope people spend less time ruminating on their pain than lower-hope individuals.37 Research is increasingly exploring the role that hope has in coping with cancer.

    • Higher Dispositional Optimism Predicts Lower Pain Reduction During Conditioned Pain Modulation

      2019, Journal of Pain
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although studies have indicated that optimism accounts for variability beyond the contribution of emotional stability, the relationship between these variables indicates a need to control for emotional stability when evaluating for the effects of optimism.32,34 Neuroticism produces sensitivity to negative stimuli, and higher neuroticism can provoke more negative affect in emotionally salient situations.14 Because painful stimuli are both negative and emotionally salient, it is unsurprising that neuroticism has been found to influence pain.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Present address: San Diego State University, Department of Public Health Hardy Tower 119, 5500 Campanile Dr., San Diego, CA 92182-4162, USA.

    View full text