Two models of personality and well-being among adolescents

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Personality is considered a major determinant of adolescents’ well-being; probably due to its relation to emotional reactivity, individual differences in intensity to responses to emotional events, and to the duration of emotional reactions. Personality, however, is a broad concept influenced by heritable traits, environment and learning characteristics. Nevertheless, most research on well-being has focused on traits models of personality. In the present study, personality was measured among 289 high school pupils from Sweden using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and the NEO Personality Inventory – Revised (NEO-PI-R). Well-being was measured as Psychological Well-Being (PWB) and Subjective Well-Being (SWB: Life Satisfaction, LS; Positive Affect, PA; and Negative Affect, NA). Participants were randomly assigned to three groups: Big Five (NEO-PI-R, SWB, and PWB), TCI (TCI, SWB, and PWB), and a Mixed group (NEO-PI-R, TCI, SWB and PWB). The results show that Neuroticism, Extraversion, Conscientiousness, Persistence, and Self-Directedness were strongly related to well-being. More importantly, in the Mixed group, Self-Directedness was strongly related to PWB, LS, and to the absence of NA while the positive relationships between Extraversion-PWB, Extraversion-PA, Neuroticism-NA and the negative relationship between Neuroticism-LS were absent. The inclusion of character as a determinant of well-being is discussed.

Introduction

Research among adults suggests that personality is a major determinant for adults’ well-being (Lyubomirsky, Sheldon, & Schkade, 2005). The relationship of personality to well-being has been investigated among adolescents and shows similar results (e.g., Fogle et al., 2002, Huebner, 1991). Personality appears to be a key element because it is related to reactivity to emotional stimuli, individual differences in intensity to responses to emotional events, and to the duration of emotional reactions (Kim-Prieto, Diener, Tamir, Scollon, & Diener, 2005). Specifically, Extraversion seems to influence well-being because it is positively related to positive emotions and being more reactive to positive affect, while Neuroticism is negatively related to negative emotions and being more reactive to negative affect (Larsen & Eid, 2008). Nevertheless, personality is a broad concept influenced by heritable traits, environment and learning characteristics (Lucas, 2008). McAdams (2001, pp. 111), for instance, suggested that personality traits may be limited to traits that “are global, stable, linear and comparative dimensions of human individuality”. Indeed, these traits are relatively similar between identical twins reared apart, although they are also influenced by environmental changes, such as becoming a parent (Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003). In contrast, heritable traits together with learning experiences from different life events may be responsible for other levels of personality (McAdams, 2001), such as the development of personal goals, values and even defence mechanisms (i.e., what people make of themselves intentionally or their character; Cloninger, 2004).

However, although the concept of character holds a major position in psychology (e.g., Allport, 1955, Rogers, 1959), most research on adolescents’ well-being has focused on traits models of personality (e.g., the five-factor model or Big Five; Costa & McCrae, 1992). Although character might be influenced by basic traits (e.g., neurotics might develop more defence mechanisms while extraverts might rely more on social relationships; Haidt, 2006), it is also influenced by changes in one’s life. In this context, character probably modifies the significance or meaning of what is experienced, in turn, influencing well-being (Cloninger, 2004). In other words, investigating the relationship between personality and well-being using only trait models of personality needs to be reconsidered. Moreover, in the field of positive psychology, well-being research complements measures of physical (e.g. health) and material (e.g. income) well-being with assessments of optimal psychological functioning and experience (Ryan & Deci, 2001). Two approaches have been distinctive across studies. First, subjective well-being (SWB; Diener, 1984) which focuses on assessment of individuals’ own judgements about life satisfaction (LS), the frequency of positive affects (PA) and the infrequency of negative affects (NA). The second is psychological well-being (PWB; Ryff, 1989) which includes six distinct constructs: autonomy, personal growth, self-acceptance, life purpose, mastery, and the ability to have positive relations with others. These six constructs define PWB both theoretically and operationally, and they identify what promotes effective adaptation to life events and emotional and physical health (Ryff, 1989).

The proposition in the present study is that character needs to be included when researchers investigate the relationship between personality and well-being. This might be necessary, not only due to the definition of personality per se, but also due to recent research suggesting that cognitive intrapersonal characteristics such as locus of control (Ash & Huebner, 2001), self-efficacy (Fogle et al., 2002), and adaptive attribution style for positive events (Rigby & Huebner, 2005) are related to specific measures of well-being (life satisfaction). These intrapersonal characteristics are, at least in part, a good definition of character. Moreover, the present study addresses other measures of well-being (i.e., positive affect, negative affect & psychological well-being) that until recently are gaps in the adolescence literature (for some studies addressing these measures see among others: Garcia and Siddiqui, 2009a, Garcia and Siddiqui, 2009b, Lewis et al., 2009). The understanding of character as a determinant of well-being in adolescence is important – adolescents that experience high levels of life satisfaction also show less emotional and behavioral problems (Suldo & Huebner, 2006), and positive emotions during school are related to higher student engagement and adaptive coping (Lewis et al., 2009). Additionally, traits are considered to be 40–60% heritable and relatively stable across the life span, the heritability is greatest for Extraversion and Neuroticism (Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001). In contrast, character increases with age – especially between adolescence and early adulthood (Cloninger, Svrakic, & Przybeck, 1993).Thus, the study of the relationship between character and well-being in the context of adolescence might lead to interventions that target specific character constructs that are malleable and important for adolescents’ well-being.

In order to test the suggestion about the need of character in the investigation of well-being, adolescents in the present study self-reported personality in two different models of personality: the Five-Factor Model or Big Five (The NEO Personality Inventory – Revised, NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae’ 1992) and Cloninger’s Psychobiological Model of Personality (The Temperament and Character Inventory, TCI; Cloninger et al., 1993). In addition, adolescents reported well-being in two models: SWB and PWB.

Section snippets

Participants

Pupils at two different high schools in the county of Blekinge, Sweden, participated in the study. The whole population from both schools (N = 300) was contacted. The adolescents were randomly assigned to three different groups: Big Five (personality measured by the Big Five and both SWB and PWB), TCI (personality measured by the TCI and both SWB and PWB), and a Mixed group (personality measured by the Big Five and the TCI and both SWB and PWB). The composition of the groups was as follows: 91

Results and discussion

For each group, four multiple regression analyses (MRA) were conducted (i.e., one MRA for each well-being measure) in order to test the personality variables that predicted well-being. Age and gender were also included in the analysis along the personality variables as predictors.

Concluding remarks

The findings support the suggestion about the inclusion of character as an important determinant of adolescents’ well-being. Nevertheless, the present study was based on self-reports. Longitudinal studies that focus on the transition from adolescence to adulthood are needed in order to truly answer the question of character as a determinant of well-being. Studies among children could also supply important findings that can be of use in the study of adolescents’ well-being. Mainly, because

References (41)

  • G.W. Allport

    Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality

    (1955)
  • C. Ash et al.

    Environmental events and life satisfaction reports of adolescents: A test of cognitive mediation

    School Psychology International

    (2001)
  • T.J. Bouchard et al.

    Genes, evolution, and personality

    Behavior Genetics

    (2001)
  • P.J. Clarke et al.

    Measuring psychological well-being in the Canadian study of health and aging

    International Psychogeriatrics

    (2001)
  • C.R. Cloninger

    Feeling good: The science of well-being

    (2004)
  • C.R. Cloninger

    Fostering spirituality and well-being in clinical practice

    Psychiatric Annals

    (2006)
  • C.R. Cloninger et al.

    A Psychobiological model of temperament and character

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1993)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    NEO PI-R professional manual

    (1992)
  • E. Diener

    Subjective well-being

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1984)
  • E.H. Erikson

    Identity: Youth and crisis

    (1968)
  • L.M. Fogle et al.

    The relationship between temperament and life satisfaction in early adolescence. Cognitive and behavioral mediation models

    Journal of Happiness Studies

    (2002)
  • D. Garcia et al.

    Adolescents’ affective temperaments: Life satisfaction, interpretation and memory of events

    The Journal of Positive Psychology

    (2009)
  • D. Garcia et al.

    Adolescents’ psychological well-being and memory for life events: Influences on life satisfaction with respect to temperamental dispositions

    Journal of Happiness Studies

    (2009)
  • J. Haidt

    The happiness hypothesis: Finding modern truth in ancient wisdom

    (2006)
  • E.S. Huebner

    Correlates of life satisfaction in children

    School Psychology Quarterly

    (1991)
  • E.S. Huebner et al.

    Life satisfaction in children and youth: Empirical foundations and implications for children and youth

    Psychology in the Schools

    (2004)
  • H.A.K. Jacques et al.

    A test of the tripartite model of anxiety and depression in elementary and high school boys and girls

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2004)
  • A.E. Kelley et al.

    Risk taking and novelty seeking in adolescence. Introduction to Part I

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2004)
  • C. Kim-Prieto et al.

    Integrating the diverse definitions of happiness: A time-sequential framework of subjective well-being

    Journal of Happiness Studies

    (2005)
  • R.J. Larsen et al.

    Ed Diener and the science of subjective well-being

  • Cited by (95)

    • Looking beyond the Big Five: A selective review of alternatives to the Big Five model of personality

      2021, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, regarding relationships to personality disorders, one study demonstrated that dimensions of the psychobiological model are better predictors (Svrakic, Whitehead, Przybeck, & Cloninger, 1993), another study showed that the Big 5 traits were more strongly related (Ball, Tennen, Poling, Kranzler, & Rounsaville, 1997), meanwhile De Fruyt, De Clercq, Van de Wiele, and Van Heeringen (2006) found that dimensions within each model were better predictors of different disorders. A study examining the influence of both models on types and elements of well-being found that dimensions within both models remained significant predictors when assessed simultaneously, though many of the expected Big 5 relationships were not supported (Garcia, 2011). One study however showed that the character trait Self-directedness explained variance in internet addiction over Big 5 traits and other relevant variables (Montag et al., 2011).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text