The development of an adolescent measure of EI

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.009Get rights and content

Abstract

The purpose of this paper was to evaluate the reliability and validity of a modified adolescent version of the Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (Adolescent SUEIT). Study 1 identified qualitative changes and rewording of items necessary to make the SUEIT more ‘adolescent friendly’. In Study 2 the adolescent modified version of the SUEIT was administered to a larger sample of adolescents (N = 1002) to gather information on internal reliability, and to perform validity testing via exploratory factor analysis. The sub-scale reliability of the Adolescent SUEIT was found to be moderate to high, and a four-factor model was most representative of the adolescent sample. It was also noted that the more basic emotional intelligence (EI) abilities were positively related to age; females reported higher levels of emotional perception; and mean scores for the adolescent sample were below the norm for an adult population. With the amount of EI research with adolescents increasing, it is important to have valid and reliable tests available for research into important social and educational outcomes for adolescents. This initial evidence suggests the Adolescent SUEIT is a reliable and valid tool, and should be used to further understand the role of EI in adolescence, and how EI relates to important life criteria.

Introduction

Emotions are internal events that coordinate many psychological sub-systems including physiological responses, cognition, and conscious awareness. They provide an emotional valence for a person’s changing relationships, helping to provide importance and meaning to an individual’s environment. The ability to understand one’s emotional states or emotional problems is an important indicator of healthy mental functioning (Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002). Thus, it has been proposed that the concept of intelligence should be expanded to include “emotional intelligence” (EI), generally defined as an ability to recognise the meanings of emotions and their relationships, and to reason and problem-solve on the basis of these emotions (Bar-On, 1997, Ciarrochi et al., 2001a, Cooper and Sawaf, 1997, Goleman, 1995, Mayer and Salovey, 1997, Palmer and Stough, 2001, Salovey et al., 1993, Salovey and Mayer, 1990).

The current conceptualisation of EI was originally proposed by Salovey and Mayer (1990); it offers perhaps the most promising theory of EI, considering both emotions and cognition equally (Mayer & Salovey, 1993). According to Mayer and Salovey (1997), EI reflects a composite of abilities: perceiving, understanding, reasoning and the regulation of emotions. Emotional intelligence was popularised by Goleman’s (1995) best-seller “Emotional Intelligence”, and since has headlined many popular books (e.g. Cooper and Sawaf, 1997, Gottman, 1997, Salerno, 1996, Segal, 1997). Amidst this excitement, a number of researchers attempted to develop self-report measures of EI or emotional intelligence-related constructs (Bar-On, 1997, Goleman, 1995, Salovey and Mayer, 1990, Schutte et al., 1998). As these tests were developed, some researchers (e.g. Davies, Stankov, & Roberts, 1998) discovered that many EI measures had poor internal reliability, did not distinguish from well-established personality factors, and have unstable factorial validity. More recent evidence suggests that some aspects of EI can be reliably measured in adults, and can reliably predict outcomes such as leadership style (Gardner & Stough, 2002), depression (Ciarrochi et al., 2001b, Ciarrochi et al., 2000, Ciarrochi et al., 2002, Ciarrochi et al., 2001a) and life satisfaction (Palmer, Donaldson, & Stough, 2002). Gender differences have also been reported in a number of previous studies (Ciarrochi et al., 2000, Mayer and Geher, 1996, Wertlieb et al., 1987, Wierzbicki, 1989), with findings suggesting that women are superior to men in perceiving emotion.

Palmer and Stough (2001) developed a self-report measure of EI named the Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT). It provides scores on five factors: Emotional recognition and expression (in oneself) (ERE) – the ability to identify one’s own feelings and emotional states, and the ability to express those inner feeling to others; emotions direct cognition (EDC) – the extent to which emotions and emotional knowledge are incorporated in decision making and/or problem solving; understanding of emotions external (UE) – the ability to identify and understand the emotions of others; emotional management (EM) – the ability to manage positive and negative emotions within both oneself and others; and emotional control (EC) – how effectively emotional states experienced, such as anger, stress, anxiety and frustration are controlled.

Despite the encouraging initial evidence of reliable measures of EI for adults, very little research has been done with respect to adolescents. Research measuring EI in young people has predominantly utilised performance measures, such as face recognition (Cassidy et al., 1992, Custrini and Feldman, 1989) and/or observations by peers, parents or teachers (Carlson-Jones et al., 1998, Eisenberg et al., 1995, Saarni, 1999). Surprisingly, little research has examined or employed the use of self-report measures of EI in adolescents (Ciarrochi et al., 2001b, Spirito et al., 1991). Despite this lack of research, a few more recent studies have made use of adolescent EI measures, with EI being found to be related to academic success (Parker et al., 2004, Petrides et al., 2004); deviant behaviour (Petrides et al., 2004); and smoking risk factors (Trinidad, Unger, Chou, & Anderson-Johnson, 2004). This early evidence concerning the role of EI across adolescence suggests that EI does play a significant role in adolescents’ everyday life. Given that intellectual capacities grow with age and experience from childhood to early adulthood (Dempster, 1985, Fancher, 1985), it is likely that EI is not fully developed in adolescents. Thus the development of EI related skills might provide adolescents with skills to deal with the emotional difficulties (Ciarrochi et al., 2001b) of everyday life.

Ciarrochi et al. (2001b) collected data from 131 students aged 13–15 years to investigate the reliability and validity of an older self-report measure of EI (SEI, Schutte et al., 1998). They reported that scores on the SEI were internally consistent, higher in females, and that EI was positively associated with skills in identifying emotional expressions, social supports, extent of satisfaction with social support, and mood management behaviour. This finding suggests that EI can be reliably measured in adolescents as young as 13 years using a self-report measure. In the present paper we examine the reliability and validity of a modified adolescent version of the SUEIT. There are a number of issues that are relevant when examining the utility of a psychometric scale developed for adults (e.g. the SUEIT), for use in adolescent samples. Firstly, the scale was initially developed for adult administration and requires a reasonably good level of verbal fluency, suggesting that the language of some items may need to be changed. Secondly, the developmental nature of EI in adolescents may result in the adult five-factor model of the SUEIT not being applicable for adolescents. Emotional intelligence is said to be developmental in nature (Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 2000), therefore, it is expected that EI as measured by the adolescent modified SUEIT will increase with age. Further, given that gender differences in EI have been reported in adults it is expected that similar gender differences will be found in adolescents.

To develop the SUEIT into a reliable and valid measure of EI for adolescent administration, two studies were conducted. In study 1, qualitative changes were identified and rewording of items was performed to make them more ‘adolescent friendly’. In study 2 the adolescent modified version of the SUEIT was administered to a larger sample of adolescents, to gather information on reliability, and to perform factorial validity testing.

Section snippets

Participants

Two independent samples were employed. An initial group were asked to indicate their understanding of the items (T1) and a follow-up group were presented with reworded items (T2). T1 participants included 15 female (M age = 15.27 years, SD = 1.03, range 14–17 years) and 20 male (M age = 14.50 years, SD = 0.95, range 13–16 years) public high school students. T2 participants included 23 female (M age = 15.52 years, SD = 0.73, range 14–17 years) and 29 male (M age = 15.34 years, SD = 0.97, range 13–17 years)

T1 results

The purpose of T1 was to identify language in items that adolescents may not understand. Frequency tables were produced to determine the percentage of participants that understand each item. Items with less than 80% understanding were considered to be a problem and were reworded. Table 1 displays the items with less than 80% understanding.

T2 results

Frequency tables were produced for each item to determine the percentages of students’ understanding of each item. Table 2 displays the items with less than

Study 2

Study 2 specifically focused on whether the newly revised Adolescent SUEIT was reliable, and if the 5-factor model of EI as suggested by Palmer and Stough (2001) would hold for adolescents. To conduct this study a large adolescent sample was given the Adolescent SUEIT. Data was then analysed for reliability and validity.

Results

The 64 items of the Adolescent SUEIT were item analysed and unreliable items were excluded from further analysis. The 57 remaining items of the Adolescent SUEIT were then factor analysed. Though the original SUEIT comprised of five factors, a scree plot supported a four or five-factor solution. The four-factor model was the only model that showed simple structure, and made theoretical sense. A principal-axis factor analysis was conducted setting the number of factors to four; an oblique

General discussion

The purpose of the current study was to determine the suitability of the SUEIT for measuring EI in adolescents. Of concern was the item language, particularly the verbal fluency required to understand what was being assessed. Study 1 showed that there was a problem with item language, with 28 of the 64 items failing to be understood by an adolescent sample. This issue was overcome after rewording the items in T2, as such, it was suggested that the modified adolescent items could be understood

References (39)

  • J. Cassidy et al.

    Family-peer connections: the roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children’s understanding of emotions

    Child Development

    (1992)
  • J.V. Ciarrochi et al.

    Emotional intelligence in everyday life: A scientific inquiry

    (2001)
  • R.K. Cooper et al.

    Executive EQ: Emotional intelligence in leaders and organizations

    (1997)
  • R.J. Custrini et al.

    Children’s social competence and nonverbal encoding and decoding of emotions

    Journal of Clinical Child Psychology

    (1989)
  • M. Davies et al.

    Emotional intelligence: in search of an elusive construct

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • F.N. Dempster

    Short-term memory development in childhood and adolescence

  • L.A. Downey et al.

    Examining the relationship between leadership, emotional intelligence, and intuition in senior female managers

    Leadership and Organization Development Journal

    (2006)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: a longitudinal study

    Child Development

    (1995)
  • R.E. Fancher

    The intelligence men, makers of the IQ controversy

    (1985)
  • Cited by (40)

    • Differentiating the contributions of emotional intelligence and resilience on adolescent male scholastic performance

      2019, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      EI has been modeled as either a distinct group of mental abilities, that is, as a specific intelligence (Ability EI) or as a combination of positive traits (trait EI), such as, self-esteem, optimism and hapiness (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2008; Petrides & Furnham, 2001). The current study employs a self-report scale based on the four branch Ability EI model originally posited by Salovey and Mayer (1990), the Adolescent Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT-A; Luebbers, Downey, & Stough, 2007). The scale used has been shown repeatedly to provide reliable and valid measurements in adolescents, predicting several key scholastic outcomes in adolescents previously (such as scholastic success, coping and bullying behaviours (Downey et al., 2008; Downey et al., 2014; Downey, Johnston, Hansen, Birney, & Stough, 2010; Lomas, Stough, Hansen, & Downey, 2012; Luebbers et al., 2007).

    • Green teens: Investigating the role of emotional intelligence in adolescent environmentalism

      2019, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Third, this convenience sample comprised predominantly male adolescents (76%) from suburban independent schools. Previous studies have reported that females tend to score higher on certain EI dimensions (Downey et al., 2010; Luebbers et al., 2007), as well as on measures of environmentalism (Boeve-De Pauw et al., 2011); however, gender differences in NEP, PEBS and SUEIT scores were minimal, and gender differences were statistically controlled. Fourth, the geographical location of this sample may have influenced the capacity to perform PEBs (Stern, Dietz, & Guagnano, 1995).

    • The validity of the Wong and Law Emotional Intelligence Scale in a Chinese sample: Tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences across gender and age

      2017, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, Mayer, Caruso, and Salovey (1999) found that adults scored significantly higher than adolescents. This pattern of results is also found in many other studies (Bar-On, 1997; Luebbers et al., 2007; Sliter, Chen, Withrow, & Sliter, 2013). However, it is important to note that, when examining these age differences, we must assure that the WLEIS does measure the same latent structure across different age groups.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text