Asymmetrical number-space mapping in the avian brain

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.012Get rights and content

Abstract

When trained to peck a selected position in a sagittally-oriented series of identical food containers, and then required to generalize to an identical series rotated by 90°, chicks identify as correct only the target position from the left end, while choosing the right one at chance. Here we show that when accustomed to systematic changes in inter-elements distances during training or faced with similar spatial changes at test, chicks identify as correct both the target positions from left and right ends. However, ordinal position is spontaneously encoded even when inter-element distances are kept fixed during training (in spite of the fact that distances between elements suffice for target identification without any numerical computation). We explain these findings in terms of intra-hemispheric coupling of bilateral numerical (ordinal) representation and unilateral (right hemispheric) spatial representation of the number line, producing differential allocation of attention in the left and right visual hemifields.

Research highlights

► The ms clarifies the nature of already described avian pseudoneglect-like behaviour. ► Numerical information is spontaneously encoded by chicks. ► Numerical (ordinal) computation would be bilateral (both hemispheres). ► Spatial information is processed unilaterally (into the right hemisphere). ► The left-ward bias depends on coupling of numerical and spatial representations.

Introduction

An important aspect of numerical cognition is the ability to represent ordinal (serial) relationships, which refers to the ability to identify an object on the exclusive basis of its position in a series of identical objects. Rats are capable of learning to enter a target tunnel solely on the basis of its ordinal position in an array of 6 (Davis & Bradford, 1986) or 18 (Suzuki & Kobayashi, 2000) tunnels. Honey bees are able to find a food source located between the 3rd and the 4th position along a series of 4 identical, equally spaced landmarks (Chittka & Geiger, 1995), they can also identify the 4th position in a series of 5, and generalize it to a novel series of objects (Dake & Srinivasan, 2008).

Young domestic chicks (Gallus gallus) also show sophisticated serial ordering abilities (for a review see Vallortigara, Regolin, Chiandetti, & Rugani, 2010). Week old chicks trained to peck either at the 3rd, 4th, or 6th position in a series of 10 identically spaced locations, sagittally aligned in front of the birds’ starting point, learned accurately to identify such positions (Rugani, Regolin, & Vallortigara, 2007). Control experiments ruled out the possibility that chicks were using distance as a cue. A serendipitous bias for the left hemispace was, however, found in one of the experiments controlling for spatial cues. Chicks, once trained to peck a target position in a sagittally-oriented series of identical elements, were then required to generalize to an identical series, but rotated by 90°. Following rotation, the series was oriented fronto-parallel with respect to the chick, hence the correct position could not be located on the basis of absolute distances from the starting point. Moreover, as both ends were at a same distance from the birds’ starting point, the correct position could be identified either from the left or from the right end. Interestingly, chicks identified as correct only the 4th position from the left end, and not the 4th from the right end, which was chosen at chance.

In subsequent work (Rugani, Kelly, Szelest, Regolin, & Vallortigara, 2010), the same behaviour was also described in a different species of birds, i.e. adult nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana), as well as domestic chicks, using a larger series, which comprised 16 (rather than 10) identical and aligned positions. When, at test, the series was rotated by 90° as compared to training, lying fronto-parallel to the bird’s starting position, both species showed a bias for identifying selectively the correct position (either the 4th or the 6th element for separate groups of subjects) from the left but not from the right end.

The preference for targets located on the left hemispace may be due to a bias in the allocation of attention, somewhat similar to that shown by humans and dubbed as “pseudoneglect” (Albert, 1973, Jewell and McCourt, 2000). In fact, “pseudoneglect” phenomena have been described in birds with selective allocation of attention to the left hemifield during free foraging (Diekamp, Regolin, Vallortigara, & Güntürkün, 2005) and in bisection tasks (Diekamp, Manns, et al., 2005, Regolin, 2006). Somewhat similar phenomena of pseudoneglect favouring the left hemifield have been described also for amphibians (Vallortigara, Rogers, Bisazza, Lippolis, & Robins, 1998; and see for general reviews MacNeilage et al., 2009, Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005).

The birds’ bias is also reminiscent of the human mental number line phenomenon. As early as 1880, Galton showed that humans describe and think of numbers as being represented on a mental number line, which is usually oriented from left to right. Modern research provides evidence that number magnitude may be represented on a left-to-right oriented number line (Dehaene, 1997). It remains to be shown, however, whether the spatial orientation of the human mental number line is entirely acquired culturally (i.e. it may be linked to writing and reading rules) or if it depends, at least in part, on biologically specific biases in the allocation of attention in extra-corporeal space. Typically human studies on the mental number line are associated with aspects of number cognition which rely on the ability to represent ordinal relations, i.e. mastering the rule that when one element is added to a given set, the new set becomes larger than the previous one and smaller than the next (see for instance the studies on the so-called SNARC effect, see Dehaene, Bossini, & Giraux, 1993). A simpler and more basic ability is that of identifying an object on the exclusive basis of its position in a series of identical objects, without any association with a magnitude of the distances among ordinal positions. Such an ability may provide a viable strategy for investigating biases in space-number mapping in non-linguistic and non-cultural creatures.

In the present series of experiments we aimed to investigate more precisely the nature of the left-hemi-space bias exhibited by birds during serial ordering tasks. In the experiments by Rugani et al. (2010) birds performed successfully at test, when some of the non-numerical cues available during training, such as distance from the starting point and walking time, could no longer be of any use to solve the task. However, birds could still rely on the distance of the correct position from the beginning of the series as a spatial cue to identify the target. The first experiment was aimed to investigate this issue, by disrupting this source of spatial information, manipulating the distance between the positions in the series, both during training and during the test.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

Previous work demonstrated that young chicks (G. gallus) are able to use ordinality to identify a target element (the 3rd, the 4th or the 6th) in a series of 10 identical and equally spaced elements, sagittally oriented with respect to the chicks’ starting point. A peculiar finding was that whenever, during a subsequent generalization test, the target position had to be identified on a left/right oriented series, chicks would refer the correct position starting from the left end of the series

Experiment 2

In Experiment 1, the inter-elements distances were changed, from trial to trial, both during training and testing, and therefore chicks could use the serial position (number) of the elements but could not rely on spatial discrimination (neither in terms of absolute nor relative distances among the elements of the series). The lack of any left–right bias in such Experiment 1, in which coding of spatial information was prevented, seems to suggest that the left bias observed in previous works

Experiment 3

In Experiment 2 the serial position of the elements was correctly identified by chicks at test, suggesting that numerical information coded during training with fixed spatial locations can be used, and is actually preferentially used with the fronto-parallel series. In fact, when at test chicks were faced with a conflict between inter-element distances, as learned during training, and correct ordinal position, they clearly selected the correct ordinal position. Curiously, chicks did not select

Experiment 4

The results of Experiment 3 clearly showed that a change in colour (i.e. a change not involving a spatial manipulation) in the fronto-parallel oriented series did not affect the presence of the left-ward bias. It is possible to conclude that so far, the bias has been observed whenever congruency was respected in spatial-numerical information at test as compared to the series experienced at training, in spite of featural changes. Whereas any spatial changes should be responsible for bias

Experiment 5

If the right hemisphere, which is likely to produce the left hemi-space bias, takes into account absolute and relational properties associated with the distance between the target and the endpoints with respect to the overall length of the series, then one could imagine that accustoming chicks to systematic changes in these parameters during training would also produce a reduction of the left bias. This hypothesis was tested in Experiment 5.

General discussion

Asymmetries in our serial ordering test can be straightforwardly accounted for by a model that assumes differential encoding, processing or integration by the two hemispheres for spatial and numerical information.

The previously documented tendency to start to “count” from left to right (Rugani et al., 2010) seems to be strictly related to the possibility to use at test the spatial information experienced during training. Whenever this is not the case (in Experiments 1 and 2), chicks choose both

Acknowledgments

All procedures were in accordance with Italian and European Community laws on animal research and treatment.

This research is part of the project EDCBNL (Evolution and Development of Cognitive, Behavioural and Neural Lateralization—2006/2009), supported by the Commission of the European Communities within the framework of the specific research and technological development programme “Integrating and strengthening the European Research Area” (initiative “What it Means to be Human”), through a

References (23)

  • B. Diekamp et al.

    Line bisection error and pseudoneglect in birds

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2005)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text