Elsevier

Neuroscience

Volume 303, 10 September 2015, Pages 462-473
Neuroscience

Adult attachment style modulates neural responses in a mentalizing task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.062Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The neural correlates of mentalizing were investigated using an interactive task.

  • Impact of adult attachment styles “avoidance” (AV) and “anxiety” (ANX) was examined.

  • AV was positively and ANX negatively correlated with task-associated neural activity.

  • Avoidantly attached adults activated brain areas implicated in emotion regulation.

Abstract

Adult attachment style (AAS) is a personality trait that affects social cognition. Behavioral data suggest that AAS influences mentalizing proficiency, i.e. the ability to predict and explain people’s behavior with reference to mental states, but the neural correlates are unknown. We here tested how the AAS dimensions “avoidance” (AV) and “anxiety” (ANX) modulate neural correlates of mentalizing.

We measured brain activation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in 164 healthy subjects during an interactive mentalizing paradigm (Prisoner’s Dilemma Game). AAS was assessed with the Relationship Scales Questionnaire, including the subscales AV and ANX.

Our task elicited a strong activation of the mentalizing network, including bilateral precuneus, (anterior, middle, and posterior) cingulate cortices, temporal poles, inferior frontal gyri (IFG), temporoparietal junctions, superior medial frontal gyri as well as right medial orbital frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus (MFG), and amygdala. We found that AV is positively and ANX negatively correlated with task-associated neural activity in the right amygdala, MFG, midcingulate cortex, and superior parietal lobule, and in bilateral IFG. These data suggest that avoidantly attached adults activate brain areas implicated in emotion regulation and cognitive control to a larger extent than anxiously attached individuals during mentalizing.

Introduction

“Avoidance” (AV) and “anxiety” (ANX) are important dimensions of the adult attachment style (AAS; Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007), which is relevant for social behavior and cognition. That individual differences in AAS are related to neural correlates of mentalizing is likely, but has not been tested so far. With this study we investigated whether AV and ANX are associated with distinct activations in a mentalizing-related neural network.

An important framework for the understanding of human social-cognitive development is offered by attachment theory. It proposes that humans have an innate motivation to form an emotional bond with another person (attachment figure) and that its evolutionary function is to give protection and care for one’s child (Bowlby, 1969). In a social constructionist view the function of attachment may go far beyond providing physical protection to the child. Attachment is assumed to foster social cognition and thereby to prepare the developing child for cooperation with others (Fonagy et al., 2007).

Caregivers differ in the way they respond to the proximity-seeking behavior of the child and thereby shape its attachment behavior. The adaptation to repeated contacts leads to cognitive-affective structures (internal working models) on the availability and security-providing features of the attachment figure. Thereby the individual attachment style evolves over time into an individual trait that remains moderately stable into adulthood (Fraley, 2002) and affects social cognition and behavior (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007). The individual attachment style is “activated” not only in close relationships, but also toward unfamiliar others (to avoid confusion with “brain activation” in the remainder of the text we replace “activate” with “trigger” and “deactivate” with “disable” when referring to the attachment system) (Fraley et al., 2006, Vrtička et al., 2008, Vrtička et al., 2012). A secure attachment style is developed in a history of responsive and trustful interaction experiences. Attachment figure unavailability however leads to insecure attachment and forces a person to use a “secondary attachment strategy” (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007). At least two different dimensions of attachment style exist that are termed “avoidance” (AV) and “anxiety” (ANX). In adulthood AV and ANX can be reliably assessed by two-dimensional self-report measures, conceptualized as two orthogonal axes (Simpson et al., 1992, Kurdek, 2002, Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007).

An avoidant attachment style (AV) is characterized by disabling strategies such as maintaining of self-reliance and distance, and by avoiding emotional states that might trigger the attachment system. Inhibited attention to emotions of oneself and others, down-regulated emotions, and blocked emotional reactions are typical for AV, as well as enhanced attention to environmental objects and predominance of cognitive information processing (Crittenden, 1995, Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007). By contrast, an anxious attachment style (ANX) is characterized by exaggerated proximity-seeking, heightened access and attention to threat-related memories and thoughts, and a predominance of affective communication (Crittenden, 1995, Mikulincer and Shaver, 2007). Thus, the individual attachment styles represent important personality dimensions which affect social-cognitive and behavioral processes in social situations.

Few neuroimaging studies investigated neural correlates of AAS by means of affective and/or attachment-related stimuli. In avoidantly attached individuals the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and insula were less activated following the experience of social exclusion vs. social inclusion in a virtual ball-tossing game (DeWall et al., 2012). Positive feedback provided by pictures of happy facial expressions in a pseudogame context resulted in less activation of reward-related areas (ventral striatum, ventral tegmental area) in avoidantly attached subjects (Vrtička et al., 2008). Activation of somatosensory cortices was negatively correlated with AV during processing of sad facial expressions (Suslow et al., 2009). These data support the view that the strategy of withdrawal renders avoidant individuals less sensitive to social rejection, negative interpersonal signals, and to social reward. However in response to pictures of unpleasant social scenes and facial signals implying social conflict AV was positively associated with recruitment of brain areas related to cognitive control as well as cognitive and emotional conflict (dACC, ventral ACC), and emotion regulation (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) (Vrtička et al., 2012). In response to pictures of pleasant social scenes AV was positively correlated with brain activity in regions implicated in motor inhibition and valuation (supplemental motor area, medial orbitofrontal cortex) (Vrtička et al., 2012). During negative thought suppression avoidantly attached individuals displayed higher activation of the ventral anterior cingulate cortex and lateral prefrontal cortex which was interpreted as less efficient suppression of negative thoughts (Gillath et al., 2005). Altogether these data suggest that AV modulates activation of brain areas implicated in pain, conflict and reward processing, and emotion regulation (e.g. dACC, insula, lateral prefrontal cortex, and ventral striatum) during the processing of emotionally significant cues.

ANX was shown to be associated with greater neural activations in dACC in response to experience of social rejection and during thinking about negative relationship scenarios (Gillath et al., 2005, DeWall et al., 2012). Negative feedback provided by angry faces and pictures of unpleasant social scenes elicited higher activation of amygdala in anxiously attached individuals (Vrtička et al., 2008, Vrtička et al., 2012). However facial cues depicting sadness did not evoke ANX-associated neural activations (Donges et al., 2012). Anxiously attached individuals further recruited several brain areas implicated in emotion processing and memory to a higher extent when they were confronted with social rejection experience, happy facial expressions, and pictures of pleasant or unpleasant social scenarios (DeWall et al., 2012, Donges et al., 2012, Vrtička et al., 2012). They activated brain areas implicated in emotion regulation (orbitofrontal cortex) to a lower extent when thinking about negative relationship scenarios (Gillath et al., 2005). In sum, the results in anxiously attached individuals show differential activation in the dACC, amygdala, and hippocampus (among others) which were interpreted as heightened vigilance, salience, and memory for emotionally significant social cues.

These previous neuroimaging studies focused on automatic brain reactivity to facial expressions, thought suppression, emotion processing, and response to social exclusion vs. inclusion. To our knowledge, the effect of AAS on the neural correlates of mentalizing has not been addressed so far. However, behavioral data have shown links between attachment quality and mentalizing ability (see below).

Mentalizing, also called theory-of-mind, is defined by “imputing mental states to oneself and others” (Premack and Woodruff, 1978) and provides “the ability to predict and explain people’s behavior with reference to mental states” (Repacholi and Slaughter, 2003). Mentalizing ability is regarded as fundamental for successful human social interactions and is typically impaired in severe mental disorders like autism (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985) and schizophrenia (Biedermann et al., 2012).

Mentalizing is regarded as a multidimensional construct and likely comprises distinct cognitive processes and cerebral networks (Frith and Frith, 2001, Hynes et al., 2006, Fonagy and Luyten, 2009). Most commonly a distinction between cognitive and affective mentalizing is made with respect to the content of mentalizing that can be either knowledge and beliefs or emotions and intentions (Brothers and Ring, 1992, Fonagy and Luyten, 2009, Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011). A mentalizing-related neural network has been identified (Mar, 2011), that consists of the bilateral medial prefrontal cortices, temporoparietal junctions, superior temporal sulci, temporal poles (TPs), anterior temporal lobes, posterior cingulate cortices, precuneus, inferior frontal gyrus, and possibly the amygdala (of the right hemisphere in nonstory-based studies). Neural cognitive and affective execution loops for the processing of affective and cognitive mental states have been put forward by Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory (2011). The cognitive mentalizing network is suggested to involve the dorsal parts of lateral (dlPFC) and medial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), TP, and striatum. The affective mentalizing network is assumed to engage the amygdala, inferior lateral frontal cortex (ILFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), ACC, TP, and striatum (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011).

Different approaches have been employed to investigate the neural correlates of mentalizing, categorized in story- and nonstory-based studies (Mar, 2011). Story-based designs have been criticized to be confounded by incidental executive and language-processing demands (Apperly et al., 2004). Additional concerns exist about the processing of fictional social agents in comparison to actual social agents (Mar, 2011). By contrast, interactive games like the iterative Prisoner’s Dilemma Game (PDG) – used in nonstory-based approaches – are expected to “model a real-life social situation” (Rilling et al., 2004, Krach et al., 2008, Kircher et al., 2009, Rilling et al., 2012). The use of the PDG allows for the implicit detection of mentalizing processes, including both affective and cognitive mentalizing.

Behavioral studies showed that mentalizing ability in childhood is influenced by attachment security to mother (Fonagy et al., 1997, Meins et al., 1998, Symons and Clark, 2000, De Rosnay and Harris, 2002) and to father (Fonagy et al., 2007) and that affective mentalizing ability in adolescence is modulated by AAS (Huenefeldt et al., 2013b). The relation of AAS and affective mentalizing in adulthood was investigated in healthy women in a behavioral study using a paradigm with stimuli from the eye region (“Reading the mind in the eyes test” = RMET) and a two-dimensional self-report measure of AAS (“Experiences in Close Relationships” questionnaire) (Huenefeldt et al., 2013a). ANX was associated with better recognition of emotionally neutral and difficult stimuli while there was no correlation with AV showing that ANX involves higher affective mentalizing proficiency. These behavioral data suggest that individual attachment styles go along with different mentalizing abilities in adulthood.

We investigated the effect of individual differences in AV and ANX on neural activity during an interactive mentalizing task, the PDG. We expected different neural activation patterns for the AAS styles “avoidance” and “anxiety” during mentalizing, considering the cognitive/affective dimensions of mentalizing and the mentalizing neural network (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011, Mar, 2011). Assuming that AV goes along with the application of cognitive strategies we predicted for AV higher activity in brain regions related to cognitive mentalizing, i.e. the dlPFC, dmPFC, dACC, dorsal TP, and dorsal striatum. For ANX we hypothesized a higher activation of regions related to affective mentalizing, i.e. the amygdala, ILFC, vmPFC, OFC, vACC, ventral TP, and ventral striatum, reflecting the ANX-associated focus on affective cues (Crittenden, 1995, Fonagy et al., 2007, Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011, Huenefeldt et al., 2013a).

Section snippets

Participants

In total, data from 164 healthy subjects (47.6% female; mean age = 23.97 years, s.d. = 3.09, range 19–35) were used for the analysis. All participants were students of the universities of Marburg or Gießen. Inclusion criteria were age (18–40 years), right-handedness (as assessed by the Edinburgh Inventory, Oldfield, 1971), German as native tongue, and Western- or Middle-European descent. Exclusion criteria were history of major psychiatric disorders of participants and their first-degree relatives

AAS – descriptive information

Avoidant attachment scores (AV) ranged from 1.25 to 5.25 with a mean value of 3.14 (SD: 0.75), anxious attachment scores (ANX) ranged from 1.00 to 6.00 with a mean value of 2.12 (SD: 0.92). AV and ANX were not correlated (r = 0.05, p = 0.50), consistent with prior results (Vrtička et al., 2012). Mean values and correlations with STAI-T and BDI scores are presented in Table 1. AV and ANX were not correlated with cooperativity (Table 1) and did not significantly differ in their correlation with

Discussion

With this study we provide first evidence, that AASs AV and ANX are distinctly associated with activations in the mentalizing-related neural network. AV was positively and ANX negatively correlated with activity in the right middle cingulate cortex (MCC), basolateral nucleus of amygdala (BLA), MFG, superior parietal lobule (SPL), and bilateral IFG. This supports our hypothesis that AV is associated with stronger engagement of cognitive mentalizing strategies than ANX. We further suggest that

Conclusion

Our findings provide first evidence that individual differences in AAS are associated with distinct patterns of neural activations in a mentalizing task. Our results support the assumption that avoidantly attached adults activate brain areas implicated in emotion regulation and cognitive control to a larger extent than anxiously attached individuals during mentalizing. This is a further step in understanding the biological underpinnings of attachment-related social interactions.

Conflicts of interest

None declared.

Acknowledgments

We thank Andreas Jansen, Jens Sommer, and Mechthild Wallnig for technical support in the preparation of and during the fMRI data acquisition. We further thank Rebecca Drexler, Johannes Krautheim, and Sabine Frenzel for their assistance during data collection. This research was supported by the LOEWE initiative funded by the State of Hesse.

References (75)

  • R. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • J.K. Rilling et al.

    The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • J.K. Rilling et al.

    Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative behavior and associated brain activity in men

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • C.-Y.C. Sylvester et al.

    Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • Ackenheil M, Stotz G, Dietz-Bauer R, Vossen A (1999). M.I.N.I. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview, German...
  • I.A. Apperly et al.

    Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: Neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands

    J Cogn Neurosci

    (2004)
  • M. Beauregard et al.

    Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion

    J Neurosci

    (2001)
  • A. Beck et al.

    Beck depression inventory: manual

    (1987)
  • F. Biedermann et al.

    Theory of mind and its relevance in schizophrenia

    Curr Opin Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • J. Bowlby

    Attachment

    (1969)
  • K.A. Brennan et al.

    Attachment styles, gender and parental problem drinking

    J Soc Personal Relationships

    (1991)
  • L. Brothers et al.

    A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds

    J Cogn Neurosci

    (1992)
  • A. Buchheim et al.

    Measuring attachment representation in an fMRI environment: a pilot study

    Psychopathology

    (2006)
  • G. Bush et al.

    Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (2002)
  • N. Canessa et al.

    The functional and structural neural basis of individual differences in loss aversion

    J Neurosci

    (2013)
  • J. Choi et al.

    The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats

    Learn Mem

    (2010)
  • N.L. Collins et al.

    Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples

    J Pers Soc Psychol

    (1990)
  • T.N. Crawford et al.

    Insecure attachment and personality disorder: a twin study of adults

    Eur J Pers

    (2007)
  • P.M. Crittenden

    Attachment and psychopathology

  • M. De Rosnay et al.

    Individual differences in children’s understanding of emotion: the roles of attachment and language

    Attach Hum Dev

    (2002)
  • C.N. DeWall et al.

    Do neural responses to rejection depend on attachment style? An fMRI study

    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci

    (2012)
  • G. Domes et al.

    The neural correlates of sex differences in emotional reactivity and emotion regulation

    Hum Brain Mapp

    (2010)
  • M.B. Donnellan et al.

    Genes, personality, and attachment in adults: a multivariate behavioral genetic analysis

    Pers Soc Psychol Bull

    (2008)
  • R.S. Edelstein et al.

    Avoiding interference: adult attachment and emotional processing biases

    Pers Soc Psychol Bull

    (2008)
  • Falissard B (2012). psy: various procedures used in psychometry. R package version...
  • P. Fonagy et al.

    A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder

    Dev Psychopathol

    (2009)
  • P. Fonagy et al.

    The relationship between belief-desire reasoning and a projective measure of attachment security (SAT)

    Br J Dev Psychol

    (1997)
  • Cited by (35)

    • Differential temporo-spatial pattern of electrical brain activity during the processing of abstract concepts related to mental states and verbal associations

      2022, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      The right temporo-parietal junction has also been identified as crucial network hub for mental perspective taking and social cognition (Martin et al., 2020; Seymour et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2016). The precuneus has been frequently associated with various forms of mentalizing (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Schneider-Hassloff et al., 2015), but seems to play a particular role in self-referential thought (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Cabeza and St Jacques, 2007). The precuneus as well as the temporal-parietal junction have been suggested to provide the input for the prefrontal mentalizing network (Abu-Akel and Shamay-Tsoory, 2011).

    • Attachment style moderates the relationship between social media use and user mental health and wellbeing

      2020, Heliyon
      Citation Excerpt :

      Firstly, despite the attempt to generalise materials to be inclusive of a larger population, this study fails to acknowledge covariates that may have affected the extent to which attachment style moderates. Commonly, previous research has revealed a range of correlates to include neurobiology (Troisi et al., 2017), cognitive and affective processes (Schneider-Hassloff et al., 2015). However, more recently the organisational effect of sex on attachment style has also been explored more extensively (Del Giudice and Angeleri, 2016).

    • A functional neuro-anatomical model of human attachment (NAMA): Insights from first- and second-person social neuroscience

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      To our knowledge, there are only two first-person social neuroimaging investigations available to date that have specifically addressed this question. A first study used a Prisoners Dilemma Game (PDG) as an interactive mentalizing paradigm during fMRI scanning (Schneider-Hassloff, Straube, Nuscheler, Wemken, & Kircher, 2015). Results revealed increased activation in avoidant participants during the PDG task in the right amygdala, middle frontal gyrus, mid-cingulate cortex, superior parietal lobule, and bilateral inferior frontal gyrus.

    • Advances in the surgical resection of temporo-parieto-occipital junction gliomas

      2019, New Techniques for Management of 'Inoperable' Gliomas
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Address: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Ulm, Steinhövel Street 5, 89075 Ulm, Germany.

    View full text