Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 124, 18 February 2019, Pages 236-245
Neuropsychologia

Age-dependent differences in the impact of paediatric traumatic brain injury on executive functions: A prospective study using susceptibility-weighted imaging

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Inhibitory control has been established sensitive to (long-term) consequences of TBI.

  • SWI is associated with parent-reported EF behaviour of adolescents.

  • Early childhood and adolescence may be vulnerable periods for TBI impact on EF.

Abstract

Childhood and adolescence represent sensitive developmental periods for brain networks implicated in a range of complex skills, including executive functions (EF; inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility). As a consequence, these skills may be particularly vulnerable to injuries sustained during these sensitive developmental periods. The present study investigated 1) whether age at injury differentially affects EF 6 months and 2 years after TBI in children aged 5–15 years, and 2) whether the association between brain lesions and EF depend on age at injury. Children with TBI (n = 105) were categorized into four age-at-injury groups based on previous studies and proposed timing of cerebral maturational spurts: early childhood (5–6 years, n = 14), middle childhood (7–9 years, n = 24), late childhood (10–12 years, n = 52), and adolescence (13–15 years, n = 15). EF were assessed with performance-based tasks and a parent-report of everyday EF. TBI patients’ EF scores 6 months and 2 years post-injury were compared to those of typically developing (TD) controls (n = 42). Brain lesions were identified using susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI). Results indicated that inhibitory control performance 2 years post-injury was differentially affected by the impact of TBI depending on age at injury. Follow-up analyses did not reveal significant differences within the age groups, preventing drawing strong conclusions regarding the contribution of age at injury to EF outcome after TBI. Tentatively, large effect sizes suggest that vulnerability is most apparent in early childhood and adolescence. Everyday inhibitory control behaviour was worse for children with TBI than TD children across childhood and adolescence at the 2-year assessment. There was no evidence for impairment in working memory or cognitive flexibility after TBI at the group level. Given small group sizes, findings from analyses into correlations between EF and SWI lesions should be interpreted with caution. Extent, number and volume of brain lesions correlated with adolescent everyday EF behaviour 6 months post-injury. Taken together, the results emphasize the need for long-term follow-up after paediatric TBI during sensitive developmental periods given negative outcomes 2-year post injury. Inhibitory control seems to be particular vulnerable to the impact of TBI. Findings of associations between EF and SWI lesions need to be replicated with larger samples.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is a common cause of childhood disability, affecting 691 per 100.000 children and adolescents per year across Western countries (Thurman, 2016). Paediatric TBI is associated with long-term cognitive impairments, with difficulties in the area of executive function (EF) being frequent and profound (Anderson and Catroppa, 2005, Babikian and Asarnow, 2009, Catroppa and Anderson, 2009, Mangeot et al., 2002, Sesma et al., 2008, van Heugten et al., 2006). EF are cognitive functions important for purposeful, goal-directed behaviour (Anderson, 2002, Diamond and Lee, 2011). They are essential for children's academic success and mental and physical health (Borella et al., 2010, Diamond, 2013, Gathercole et al., 2004). Their disruption can impact social participation and quality of life (Galvin et al., 2010, Levin et al., 2009, Levin et al., 1997, Rosema et al., 2012, Ylvisaker and Feeney, 2002). EF consist of three separable though interrelated constructs: inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility (Huizinga et al., 2006, Miyake et al., 2000). Inhibitory control refers to the ability to withhold dominant and pre-potent responses in contexts where they are not appropriate (Huizinga et al., 2006, Miyake et al., 2000). Working memory is a cognitive system which temporarily maintains and manipulates information (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974, Bayliss et al., 2005). Cognitive flexibility is the ability to shift attentional focus between tasks and mental sets (Anderson, 2002, Miyake et al., 2000). Although more severe TBI often leads to worse executive dysfunction (Anderson and Catroppa, 2005, Catroppa and Anderson, 2009, C, 1991, Anderson et al., 2009a, Woodward et al., 1999), outcomes vary widely and the relationship between injury severity and degree of EF impairment cannot yet be fully explained (Anderson and Catroppa, 2005, Catroppa and Anderson, 2009). Age at injury, as a proxy for brain and cognitive development, has received increasing attention in the literature as a potential influence on post-injury outcomes; however, research on its relationship to EF after paediatric TBI is still scarce.

Paediatric TBI occurs at a time of ongoing cognitive and neural development (Anderson and Catroppa, 2005, Anderson et al., 2005, Anderson et al., 2012, Anderson et al., 2011). The sensitive period model states that higher cognitive functions, such as EF, are particularly vulnerable when the insult occurs at times of rapid neural maturation of the function itself and its underlying networks (Anderson et al., 2011, Crowe et al., 2012, Dennis et al., 2014). For the three main EF components discussed above, development continues well into adolescence and early adulthood (Anderson, 2002, Casey et al., 2000, De Luca et al., 2003). Although each of the three EF components has a unique developmental trajectory, early and middle childhood (preschool up to approximately 9 years) has been identified as a key period for each (Anderson, 2002, Romine and Reynolds, 2005, Best et al., 2009, Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). During this stage, prior to the full maturation of EF, children have been argued to be particularly vulnerable. In support of this sensitive period model, a recent study found that children who sustained TBI in early (before 6 years) or middle childhood (7–9 years) demonstrated lower intellectual abilities than those with TBI in late childhood (10–12 years) (Crowe et al., 2012). Other studies have also reported age-dependent cognitive outcomes, including EF, in groups of children following paediatric brain injury (e.g. TBI, congenital injuries, stroke), highlighting increased vulnerability if children were injured at an age when EF were emerging (Anderson et al., 2009a, Anderson et al., 2010).

Despite protracted EF development throughout childhood and into adulthood (Anderson, 2002, Casey et al., 2000, De Luca et al., 2003), the impact of age at injury has rarely been investigated in patients beyond late childhood. Results of these studies seem to indicate that impact of TBI diminishes after early and middle childhood (Anderson, 2002, Crowe et al., 2012, Romine and Reynolds, 2005, Best et al., 2009, Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). However, the sensitive period model would predict that EF are at heightened risk for disruption during adolescence as well, since brain regions involved in these skills are undergoing rapid maturation (Giedd et al., 1999, Gogtay, 2004). In typically developing (TD) children, adolescence is identified as a sensitive period, characterised by rapid decrease in cerebral grey matter paralleled by increases in white matter, indicating synaptic pruning and myelination and resulting in functional maturation (Giedd et al., 1999, Gogtay, 2004). Immaturities in adolescent EF and underlying brain substrates are clearly apparent in, for example, enhanced risk-taking behaviour as a consequence of limited inhibitory control (Blakemore and Choudhury, 2006, Blakemore and Robbins, 2012, Sawyer et al., 2012). In the TBI literature, a recent study employed a global EF index, combining performance test scores and parent-ratings of EF, found that children with severe injuries during adolescence (13–15 years) had greater impairment than children injured during late childhood (10–12 years), and similar impairments to those injured in early or middle childhood (Krasny-Pacini et al., 2017). Adolescents showed almost no recovery over the two years post insult. Generalization of these results is difficult, however, due to small sample size and inability to determine specific EF profiles (Krasny-Pacini et al., 2017). Further investigation in larger studies is warranted, to better characterise the nature of EF impairment and its association with age at injury and injury severity.

Recent evidence suggests that EF components are supported by anatomically distributed brain networks (i.e. in frontal, temporal, parietal and subcortical regions) (Lewis et al., 2004, Monchi et al., 2006, Nowrangi et al., 2014, Power et al., 2007). A promising technique to establish a link between TBI related brain lesions and EF outcomes is susceptibility-weighted imaging (SWI). SWI makes use of a three-dimensional T2*-weighted gradient recalled echo sequence that is highly susceptible to the magnetic properties of extracellular and extravascular blood (Haacke et al., 2004, Tong et al., 2003). This technique is more sensitive than conventional imaging techniques in detecting focal as well as diffuse haemorrhagic pathology (Spitz et al., 2013, Beauchamp et al., 2011). Moreover, SWI is superior to other neuroimaging techniques such as computed tomography or conventional magnetic resonance imagining sequences in detecting micro-haemorrhages because it has increased sensitivity for revealing small traumatic axonal injuries, which may be more typical of mild TBI (Tong et al., 2003, Babikian et al., 2005, Beauchamp et al., 2011). Detecting the presence of SWI lesions can be done by visual examination, for example by radiologists, making it a useful clinical tool for diagnosis. The number and volume of lesions across the brain detected with ((sub)acute SWI analyses have been found to be predictive of general intellectual abilities from 6 months to 3 years post-injury as well as for a general neuropsychological functioning index including verbal and nonverbal memory, information processing, attention and language skills) 1–3 years post-injury (Babikian et al., 2005). The relationship between lesions detected with SWI and EF after paediatric TBI remains to be determined.

The present study extends previous research in two important ways. Firstly, given previous research has focused predominantly on severe TBI, we studied the impact of TBI across the entire spectrum of injury severity (i.e. mild, moderate, severe TBI), occurring from early childhood to adolescence. We hypothesized that, children sustaining TBI in key sensitive developmental periods including early and middle childhood and adolescence, would also demonstrate poorer EF at 6 months and 2 years post-injury. Secondly, we investigated the relations between EF outcomes after TBI and neuropathology as detected with SWI. To that end, we examined lesions in terms of extent (i.e. how many individual regions of the brain were affected, and thus how diffuse the lesions were), number and volume. We hypothesized that a greater extent, number and volume of SWI lesions would be associated with worse EF outcomes, and that the association would be stronger for children who were injured during early and middle childhood and adolescence (i.e. when the damage to the networks underlying EF occurred during a sensitive period of EF development) than for children injured during late childhood.

Section snippets

Participants

This study represents a substudy of a prospective, longitudinal cohort study of children's cognitive and social functioning after TBI (Anderson et al., 2017, Ryan et al., 2015a). Children and adolescents with TBI were recruited at time of injury on presentation to the Emergency Department of a tertiary paediatric hospital, the Royal Children's Hospital (RCH), Melbourne, Australia. Participants represented consecutive admissions to the RCH. Children and adolescents in the TD group were recruited

Sample characteristics

A total of 154 children, 112 with TBI and 42 TD controls, participated in this study. Given that our analyses pertained to children who had at least participated in the 6-month assessment, we excluded the 7 children who dropped out before the 6-month assessment from all further description. Fig. 1 shows the flow of participants throughout the study.

Demographics and injury-related details are displayed in Table 1. There were no sex differences between groups. Given differences between children

Discussion

In this study, we tested the sensitive period model for EF outcomes in a sample consisting of children and adolescents with TBI, and we explored the value of SWI in relation to EF outcomes. Consistent with the sensitive period model (Anderson et al., 2011, Crowe et al., 2012, Dennis et al., 2014) and results from previous studies (Anderson et al., 2009a, Anderson et al., 2010, Crowe et al., 2012, Krasny-Pacini et al., 2017), we hypothesized that children who sustained TBI during early or middle

Conclusions

Inhibitory control measured with a performance task as well as rated by a parent is particularly vulnerable to the long-term impact of TBI, with children with TBI scoring worse than TD children 2 years post-injury. Early childhood and adolescence seem to be developmental stages during which children are particularly vulnerable to the negative impact of brain injury, supporting a non-linear relationship between age at injury and outcome, and thus a ‘sensitive period’ model. However, these

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Victorian Neurotrauma Initiative and the Victorian Government Operational Infrastructure Scheme (No. CO6E1). The funding bodies did not play a role in the design of the study, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, or writing of the manuscript.

Declarations of interest

The authors report no conflict of interests.

References (75)

  • A. Miyake

    The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis

    Cogn. Psychol.

    (2000)
  • N.P. Ryan

    Relationships between acute imaging biomarkers and theory of mind impairment in post-acute pediatric traumatic brain injury: a prospective analysis using susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI)

    Neuropsychologia

    (2015)
  • S.M. Sawyer

    Adolescence: a foundation for future health

    Lancet

    (2012)
  • G. Teasdale et al.

    Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: a practical scale

    Lancet

    (1974)
  • P.A. Yushkevich

    User-guided 3D active contour segmentation of anatomical structures: significantly improved efficiency and reliability

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • P. Anderson

    Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood

    Child Neuropsychol.

    (2002)
  • V. Anderson

    Functional plasticity or vulnerability after early brain injury?

    Pediatrics

    (2005)
  • V. Anderson

    Does early age at brain insult predict worse outcome? Neuropsychological implications

    J. Pediatr. Psychol.

    (2009)
  • V. Anderson

    Childhood brain insult: can age at insult help us predict outcome?

    Brain

    (2009)
  • V. Anderson

    Predictors of cognitive function and recovery 10 years after traumatic brain injury in young children

    Pediatrics

    (2012)
  • V. Anderson

    Social competence at two years after child traumatic brain injury

    J. Neurotrauma

    (2017)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Recovery of executive skills following paediatric traumatic brain injury (TBI): a 2 year follow-up

    Brain Inj.

    (2005)
  • V. Anderson et al.

    Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioural plasticity after early brain insult

    Brain

    (2011)
  • V.A. Anderson

    Understanding predictors of functional recovery and outcome 30 months following early childhood head injury

    Neuropsychology

    (2006)
  • T. Babikian et al.

    Neurocognitive Outcomes and Recovery After Pediatric TBI: Meta-Analytic Review of the Literature

    (2009)
  • D.M. Bayliss

    Mapping the developmental constraints on working memory span performance

    Dev. Psychol.

    (2005)
  • M.H. Beauchamp

    Detecting traumatic brain lesions in children: ct versus MRI versus susceptibility weighted imaging (SWI)

    J. Neurotrauma

    (2011)
  • S.J. Blakemore et al.

    Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • S.-J. Blakemore et al.

    Decision-making in the adolescent brain

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2012)
  • E. Borella et al.

    The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders

    J. Learn. Disabil.

    (2010)
  • C. Catroppa et al.

    Neurodevelopmental outcomes of pediatric traumatic brain injury

    Future Neurol.

    (2009)
  • L.A. Chapman

    Clinically significant behavior problems during the initial 18 months following early childhood traumatic brain injury

    Rehabil. Psychol.

    (2010)
  • C. Coffey et al.

    Neuropsychiatric significance of subcortical encephalomalacia

    Psychopathol. Brain

    (1991)
  • J. Cohen
    (1988)
  • J. Cohen

    Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences

    (2013)
  • Colorado Medical Society

    Report of the Sports Medicine Committee: Guidelines for the Management of Concussion in Sports

    (1991)
  • L.M. Crowe

    Timing of traumatic brain injury in childhood and intellectual outcome

    J. Pediatr. Psychol.

    (2012)
  • Cited by (21)

    • Developmental divergence of structural brain networks as an indicator of future cognitive impairments in childhood brain injury: Executive functions

      2020, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is inconsistent with the idea of critical-periods of vulnerability, with no age at injury showing greater propensity to greater developmental divergence. Previous research investigating potential ‘age at injury’ effects post-TBI, do not primarily consider the magnitude of the perturbation the brain post injury (i.e. (Resch et al., 2019)). Thus, the current research opens up new opportunities in this area, offering a quantitative measure of brain perturbation (the DDI) by which we can investigate the individual and potentially interactive effects of both age at injury and magnitude of injury.

    • Pediatric traumatic brain injury and abusive head trauma

      2020, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Early age at injury is often associated with poorer outcomes and has been reported as a vulnerability factor after TBI, with increasing symptoms over time in the youngest children (Keenan et al., 2018) and more communication and problem-solving difficulties in infants, compared to toddlers (Keenan et al., 2019). However, this is not systematically the case, and seems to depend on the outcomes studied (e.g., “no strong conclusions regarding the contribution of age at injury to executive functions or behavioral outcomes”) (Le Fur et al., 2019; Resch et al., 2019). Family-related factors: A number of family/environmental factors have a strong impact on outcomes, including “socioeconomic status,” which is approached in many ways (e.g., parental education level, parental occupation, and income; Kachmar et al., 2019).

    • The Pediatric Cell Atlas: Defining the Growth Phase of Human Development at Single-Cell Resolution

      2019, Developmental Cell
      Citation Excerpt :

      These differences are not well understood at the tissue level (Fernandez et al., 2011). Age-dependent responses to anesthesia and medications (Batchelor and Marriott, 2015; Andropoulos, 2018), environmental exposures (Wright, 2017), traumatic injuries (Luerssen et al.,1988; Resch et al., 2019), or surgical outcomes (Imura et al., 2001) have been observed, but the reasons for these differences are still unclear. Childhood diseases also have age-dependent symptoms, prognoses, and outcomes (Wheeler et al., 2011) even when stratified for key clinical variables, possibly due to tissue developmental composition at the time of dysfunction (Mann et al., 2010).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text